全文获取类型
收费全文 | 389篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
397篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 32篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 21篇 |
2010年 | 9篇 |
2009年 | 10篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有397条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Despite the crucial role typically given to negative thoughts in cognitive conceptualisations of emotional problems, few studies have compared the characteristics of varieties of such thoughts. The present study compared the process features of naturally occurring worries and obsessions in a non-patient group. Analyses of variance revealed several significant differences between these types of thought. The relationship between worries and obsessions, and the clinical and conceptual importance of the observed differences is briefly discussed. 相似文献
12.
Salkovskis PM Wroe AL Gledhill A Morrison N Forrester E Richards C Reynolds M Thorpe S 《Behaviour research and therapy》2000,38(4):347-372
The cognitive-behavioural theory of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) proposes that a key factor influencing obsessional behaviour is the way in which the intrusive cognitions are interpreted. The present paper reports an investigation of links between clinical symptoms (of anxiety, depression and obsessionality) and responsibility beliefs. These beliefs include not only measures of general responsibility attitudes (assumptions) but also more specific responsibility appraisals consequent on intrusive cognitions. The characteristics of two new questionnaires specifically designed to measure these beliefs were assessed in patients suffering from Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, in patients suffering from other anxiety disorders and in non-clinical controls. The scales measuring negative beliefs about responsibility were found to have good reliability and internal consistency. Comparisons between criterion groups indicate considerable specificity for both assumptions and appraisals with respect to OCD. There was also good evidence of specificity in the association between responsibility cognitions and obsessional symptoms across groups, and that this association was not a consequence of links with anxiety or depressive symptoms. Although the two measures were correlated, they each made unique contributions to the prediction of obsessional symptoms. Overall, the results are consistent with the hypothesis that responsibility beliefs are important in the experience of obsessional problems. 相似文献
13.
14.
Sehar Ahmed Michle D. Birtel Melissa Pyle Anthony P. Morrison 《Journal of community & applied social psychology》2020,30(2):199-213
Public stigma towards people with mental health problems has been demonstrated in Western societies. Little is known about non‐Western cultures and whether cultures differ in their perceptions of people with mental health problems. Aim of this study was to examine cultural differences in prejudice, stereotypes, and discrimination towards people with psychosis. Participants were from White British and South Asian backgrounds (N = 128, aged 16–20 years) recruited from two schools and colleges in the United Kingdom. They completed a cross‐sectional survey on affective, cognitive, and behavioural dimensions of stigma. Results revealed significant cultural differences on all three stigma dimensions. South Asians attributed higher anger (prejudice) and dangerousness (stereotypes) to people with psychosis than White British. They also reported lower willingness to help, greater avoidance, and higher endorsement of segregation (discrimination). The effects of ethnic group on helping intentions, avoidance, and segregation endorsement were mediated by anger and by dangerousness. Understanding cultural differences in stigma towards psychosis will be important for designing stigma interventions as well as treatments for people with different cultural backgrounds. 相似文献
15.
The difficulty of reasoning tasks depends on their relational complexity, which increases with the number of relations that must be considered simultaneously to make an inference, and on the number of irrelevant items that must be inhibited. The authors examined the ability of younger and older adults to integrate multiple relations and inhibit irrelevant stimuli. Young adults performed well at all but the highest level of relational complexity, whereas older adults performed poorly even at a medium level of relational complexity, especially when irrelevant information was presented. Simulations based on a neurocomputational model of analogical reasoning, Learning and Inference with Schemas and Analogies (LISA), suggest that the observed decline in reasoning performance may be explained by a decline in attention and inhibitory functions in older adults. 相似文献
16.
D. L. Morrison 《Current Psychology》1988,7(2):136-156
This study examines the relationship between performance on a context-independent fault diagnosis task and two dimensions
of cognitive style: field dependence/independence (D-I) and focusing/scanning (F-S). The effects on diagnosis performance
of training and the ability of both dimensions of style jointly to predict fault-finding ability were considered. The results
showed that field independents and focusers were superior fault diagnosticians. Training, in the form of verbal feedback messages,
was uniformly effective for the D-I dimension although field independents maintained a superior level of performance, over
field dependents. In general, for the F-S dimension, training had little effect on either the focusers or scanners. A combination
of scores on the F-S and D-I dimensions significantly enhanced the proportion of the variance in testing efficiency that could
be accounted for during fault-finding. Finally it is argued that the F-S and D-I dimensions should be recognized as measures
of ability, not style. Future research should be directed at identifying between subject differences on fault diagnosis tasks
with reference to theories of information processing. 相似文献
17.
Four studies tested whether uncertainty about the self-concept can motivate people, particularly individualists who define themselves in terms of their personal traits and characteristics, to perceive their material possessions as extensions of themselves (i.e., as self-expressive). In Study 1, European American participants rated their favorite pair of blue jeans as more self-expressive after being induced to feel self-uncertain, whereas Asian American participants did not. In Study 2, participants who scored high on a measure of individualism rated their cars as more self-expressive following a self-uncertainty manipulation. In Study 3, individualists (but not collectivists) rated their favorite possessions as more self-expressive after being subject to self-uncertainty; a manipulation of self-irrelevant uncertainty did not produce these results. In Study 4, thinking about a self-expressive (relative to utilitarian) possession bolstered self-certainty among individualists, but not collectivists. Implications for research on culture, the self-concept, and possessions are discussed. 相似文献
18.
A recent paper by Kail (1988) in this journal appears to contain a significant error in the data analysis. The "goodness-of-fit" coefficients reported which suggest that overall about 94% of the variance can be accounted for by the model seem to be a substantial overestimation as a result of inappropriate procedures for statistical modeling. Using the data made available to us by Kail, we have reanalyzed these results. The corrected values range from 0.9 to 92.1% for the individual tasks with an overall average between 40 and 60%. We suggest that the support for the original conclusions is considerably weaker than reported. 相似文献
19.
Nadim N. Rouhana Anne O'Dwyer Sharon K. Morrison Vaso 《Journal of applied social psychology》1997,27(1):37-57
Employing the motivational basis of the false consensus bias, this study hypothesized that the degree of assumed similarity to in-group attitudes would differ among supporters of various political groups in intergroup conflict. We defined a conflict-maintaining bias, the adversary's extremity bias, and examined how the degree of the bias would also differ among supporters of various political groups. Data were gathered from Arab and Jewish high school and university students in Israel. Our hypotheses, that in intergroup conflict assumed similarity to in-group attitudes and the adversary's extremity bias would be greater among supporters of less conciliatory political parties than among supporters of more conciliatory political parties, were supported for one group in the conflict but not the other. Explanations of our findings are based on the differential power status of the 2 groups in the particular conflict. 相似文献
20.
Morrison AP 《Memory (Hove, England)》2004,12(4):517-524
There has been a long tradition of studying imagery in relation to psychotic symptoms. Recent studies have suggested that imagery may be involved in the development and maintenance of psychotic symptoms (hallucinations and delusions in particular). Following a review of this literature, including work conducted by the author and colleagues, a case study is used to illustrate the clinical applications of this work. Working with images that were associated with persecutory delusions appeared to contribute to a reduction in distress, preoccupation, and conviction in relation to these beliefs, which were assessed using a standardised measure (PSYRATS). The implications for theory, practice, and future research are considered. 相似文献