全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1052篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
1061篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 24篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 27篇 |
2013年 | 135篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 44篇 |
2010年 | 24篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 38篇 |
2007年 | 46篇 |
2006年 | 29篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 30篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 20篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1061条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Morris AL Harris CL 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2004,30(5):913-922
Does repetition blindness represent a failure of perception or of memory? In Experiment 1, participants viewed rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) sentences. When critical words (C1 and C2) were orthographically similar, C2 was frequently omitted from serial report; however, repetition priming for C2 on a postsentence lexical decision task was equivalent whether or not C1 was similar to C2. In Experiment 2, participants monitored RSVP sentences for a predetermined target. Participants frequently failed to detect the target when it was preceded by an orthographically similar word. In Experiment 3, the authors investigated the role of the attentional blink in this effect. These experiments suggest that repetition blindness is a failure of conscious perception, consistent with predictions of the token-individuation hypothesis. 相似文献
42.
Chaffin R Morris RK Seely RE 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2001,27(1):225-235
This study examined how readers establish the meaning of a new word from the sentence context during silent reading. Readers' eye movements were monitored while they read pairs of sentences containing a target word, context, and a word related to the target word. The target word varied in familiarity (high, low, or novel). The context varied in informativeness about the meaning of the target word (informative or neutral). The amount of time readers spent on the context depended on both the familiarity of the target word and the informativeness of the context. Readers spent additional time on the related word only when the context was neutral and the target was novel. These results indicate that readers were able to determine which areas of text were relevant and used the information to infer a meaning for the novel word. 相似文献
43.
A L Morris C L Harris 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1999,25(4):1060-1075
Six experiments used an illusory words paradigm to demonstrate that repetition blindness (RB) in orthographically similar words affects only the words' shared letters. Rapid serial visual presentation streams of words and word fragments allowed the unique letters of the 2nd critical word to combine with a subsequent fragment to create a word, as in rock shock ell. The illusory word shell was reported 2-3 times as frequently in RB conditions as in control conditions. Further experiments ruled out letter migration, contour summation, and differences in processing load as explanations for the results. These findings are inconsistent with current proposals that orthographic RB represents similarity inhibition or lexical competition or that it reflects problems with word-level token individuation. 相似文献
44.
45.
The literature on policy transfer between countries and organization has seen rapid growth. Whether framed in terms of policy
transfer, institutional transplantation, imitation and emulation or copying policy models and ideas, a sizeable number of
determinants for success have been identified to understand why such conceptual or institutional transfers grow out to become
successes or failures in their countries or organizations of adoption. What is much less common is to describe such a process
through the lens of participants inside this transfer. This paper is an attempt to do just that. It will tell the story of
the higher-educational concept of Technology, Policy and Management from Delft University of Technology in the Netherlands
to Harbin Institute of Technology in China, studied by means of participatory observation. The keys and clues for successful
transplantation from the literature will subsequently be compared with the experiences from this case, and additional lessons,
some of which are specific to Western-Chinese mutual learning, are formulated.
Martin de Jong is associate professor of policy, organization and management at the Faculty of Technology, Policy and Management
of Delft University of Technology, the Netherlands. He publishes and lectures mainly on subjects of cross-national policy
transfer, cross-cultural management and transport infrastructure policy. He is also responsible for the international master
program Engineering and Policy Analysis, for which he has helped to set up a Chinese counterpart with the same name.
Xi Bao is professor of public management and real estate management at the School of Management and the National Centre of
Technology, Policy and Management of Harbin Institute of Technology, People’s Republic of China. He is also a co-director
of the National Centre of TPM in China and lectures and publishes mainly on real estate management and infrastructure policy. 相似文献
46.
Perceived heaviness has been shown to be specific to an object’s rotational inertia (I ), its resistance to rotational acceleration.
According to the kinematic specification of dynamics (KSD) principle, we hypothesized that I is optically specified by rotational
kinematics. Using virtual depictions of wielded objects, we investigated whether the visually detected rotational kinematics
of wielded objects would influence perceived heaviness in a manner consistent with the inertial model of heaviness perception.
We scaled the virtual object’s movement so that it rotated more or less than its wielded counterpart, specifying lower and
higher I, respectively. Perceived heaviness was inversely related to the rotational scaling factor, consistent with a KSD
interpretation of the inertial model. 相似文献
47.
In January 1996, the American Board of Genetic Counseling (ABGC) adopted 27 practice-based competencies as a standard for assessing the training of graduate students in genetic counseling. These competencies were identified and refined through a collective, narrative process that took place from January through November 1994, and included directors of graduate programs in genetic counseling, ABGC board members and expert consultants. These competencies now form the basis of the document Requirements for Graduate Programs in Genetic Counseling Seeking Accreditation by the American Board of Genetic Counseling (American Board of Genetic Counseling, 1996). The competencies are organized into four domains and are presented and discussed in this article.The Consortium includes Directors of established graduate programs in genetic counseling and members of the ABGC Board of Directors who participated in the Consensus Development Conference held in January, 1994: Diane Baker (University of Michigan/ABGC); Bonnie Baty (ABGC); Joan Burns (University of Wisconsin); Debra Collins (ABGC); Virginia Corson (ABGC); Beth Fine (Northwestern University/ABGC); Elizabeth Gettig (University of Pittsburgh); Verle Headings (Howard University); Jacqueline Hecht (University of Texas); Carl Huether (University of Cincinnati); Bonnie LeRoy (University of Minnesota); Joan Marks (Sarah Lawrence College); Anne Matthews (University of Colorado); Roberta Palmour (McGill University); Lorna Phelps (Medical College of Virginia); Kimberly Quaid (Indiana University); Joan Scott (ABGC); Ann Smith (ABGC); Helen Travers (ABGC); Judith Tsipis (Brandeis University); Ann Walker (University of California-Irvine/ABGC); Jon Weil (University of California-Berkeley); S. Robert Young (University of South Carolina); Randi Zinberg (Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York). 相似文献
48.
The present studies examined the reliability and validity of the Preference Test (PT), a widely used self-report index of preferred hemisphere thinking styles. The PT consists of items that intend to tap left-hemisphere and right-hemisphere cognitions. In the first study (N=47), PT scores were found to have reasonable test–retest stability. In the second study (N=334), a factor analysis of PT scores showed that the PT has a two-factor solution that can best be interpreted in terms of putative left- and right-hemisphere items. In the third study (N=29), background EEG activity (F3, F4, P3 and P4) of subjects was recorded. PT scores were found to be related to EEG asymmetry patterns for frontal, but not for parietal, recording sites. More specifically, subjects with a left-hemisphere preference displayed relatively stronger left frontal activity than subjects with a right-hemisphere preference. By and large, the present findings provide strong support for the reliability and some suggestive evidence for the validity of the PT. 相似文献
49.
Robin G. Morris 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1987,39(3):215-227
Discrimination learning was investigated in patients with moderate to severe Alzheimer-type dementia (AD), comparing their performance with age-matched controls. Four AD patients were trained to criterion on identity matching and then shifted to the same task with novel stimuli. The AD patients showed no savings in learning to match novel stimuli, whereas a comparative group of four control subjects rapidly learned the novel matching discrimination, maintaining criterion performance on the transfer test. A second group of four patients was initially trained on oddity, taking a similar number of trials to reach criterion as the matching group. When these patients were subsequently shifted to the matching task with novel stimuli, they performed substantially worse than the first group of patients who had learned the matching task in the first stage. The lack of positive transfer in the shift between matching to matching suggests that the AD patients solved the identity-matching task on the basis of stimulus-response associations rather than a rule. The presence of negative transfer after shifting from oddity to matching may be explained by a pre-disposition to respond to a novel stimulus that is carried over into the matching task, but this warrants further investigation, as indicated in the discussion. 相似文献
50.
N. Morris S. Keane S. Calkins L. Shanahan M. O'Brien 《Journal of applied developmental psychology》2014
The present study examined the contribution of early reactivity and regulation on externalizing behavior in preadolescence. Moreover, subcomponents of attentional control (i.e., attention shifting and attention focusing) and negative reactivity (i.e., sadness and anger) were examined individually to test whether a specific combination of factors uniquely contributed to the outcome. A subset of data were utilized from the ongoing, longitudinal RIGHT Track project (N = 404), in which parents reported on individual factors at age 4 and teachers reported on externalizing behavior at age 10. A hierarchical linear regression analysis revealed a significant interaction between anger reactivity and attention shifting when controlling for early externalizing behavior, where children with high levels of anger and low levels of attention shifting experienced the greatest increase in externalizing behavior over time. An increased focus on specificity is needed in research on the interplay between reactivity and regulation in the prediction of externalizing behavior. 相似文献