全文获取类型
收费全文 | 726篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
735篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 33篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 33篇 |
2017年 | 40篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 88篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 35篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 28篇 |
2004年 | 23篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有735条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
This study examined whether automatic stereotypes captured by the implicit association test (IAT) can predict real hiring discrimination against the obese. In an unobtrusive field experiment, job applications were sent to a large number of real job vacancies. The applications were matched on credentials but differed with respect to the applicant's weight. Discriminatory behavior was quantified by the extent to which the hiring managers invited normal-weight versus obese applicants to a job interview. Several months after the behavioral data were obtained, the hiring managers completed an obesity IAT and explicit hiring preference measures. Only the IAT scores reliably predicted interview decisions. More specifically, hiring managers holding more negative automatic stereotypes about the obese were less likely to invite an obese applicant for an interview. The present research is the first to show that automatic bias predicts labor market discrimination against obese individuals. Practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
132.
Event detection is used to classify recorded gaze points into periods of fixation, saccade, smooth pursuit, blink, and noise.
Although there is an overall consensus that current algorithms for event detection have serious flaws and that a de facto
standard for event detection does not exist, surprisingly little work has been done to remedy this problem. We suggest a new
velocity-based algorithm that takes several of the previously known limitations into account. Most important, the new algorithm
identifies so-called glissades, a wobbling movement at the end of many saccades, as a separate class of eye movements. Part
of the solution involves designing an adaptive velocity threshold that makes the event detection less sensitive to variations
in noise level and the algorithm settings-free for the user. We demonstrate the performance of the new algorithm on eye movements
recorded during reading and scene perception and compare it with two of the most commonly used algorithms today. Results show
that, unlike the currently used algorithms, fixations, saccades, and glissades are robustly identified by the new algorithm.
Using this algorithm, we found that glissades occur in about half of the saccades, during both reading and scene perception,
and that they have an average duration close to 24 msec. Due to the high prevalence and long durations of glissades, we argue
that researchers must actively choose whether to assign the glissades to saccades or fixations; the choice affects dependent
variables such as fixation and saccade duration significantly. Current algorithms do not offer this choice, and their assignments
of each glissade are largely arbitrary. 相似文献
133.
Rolstad S Nordlund A Gustavsson MH Eckerström C Klang O Hansen S Wallin A 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2008,49(6):577-582
The purpose of this study was to construct a Swedish version of the National Adult Reading Test (NART-SWE), a test for assessment of premorbid IQ, and to investigate its validity and reliability on healthy controls and patients with mild Alzheimer's disease. As Swedish pronunciation rules are fixed, NART-SWE was constructed using loan words. NART-SWE has satisfactory psychometric properties: Inter-rater and retest reliability as well as internal consistency are very high. The NART-SWE demonstrates face validity. In addition, high correlation with IQ was obtained. A significant model emerged when using NART-SWE to predict IQ. Furthermore, no significant differences were observed when comparing performance for healthy controls' with that of patients with Alzheimer's disease on NART-SWE. It does appear that reading of irregular words is intact in mild Alzheimer's disease. 相似文献
134.
Pinto, Olivers, and Theeuwes (2006) showed that a static target can be efficiently found among different types of dynamically changing distractors. They hypothesized that attention employs a broad division between static and dynamic information, a hypothesis that conforms with earlier research. In the present study, we investigated whether attention can only make use of this crude division or can exploit more subtle discriminations within the dynamic domain. In Experiment 1, participants were able to efficiently find a blinking target among moving distractors and moving targets among blinking distractors, although all items changed at the same rate and produced the same change in local luminance. In Experiment 2, search for a dynamic target among dynamic distractors was aided when we gave the distractors additional dynamic cues. Experiment 3 showed that making the displays equiluminant affected search efficiency for a static target among moving distractors, but not among blinking distractors. The findings refute the broad division hypothesis and suggest that object continuity plays an important role in selection. 相似文献
135.
Per Lindström 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2006,35(3):225-230
We present a quite simple proof of the fixed point theorem for GL. We also use this proof to show that Sambin's algorithm
yields a fixed point. 相似文献
136.
Broström E Ortqvist M Haglund-Akerlind Y Hagelberg S Gutierrez-Farewik E 《Human movement science》2007,26(2):296-305
Motion of the body center of mass (CoM) can often indicate the overall effect of the strategy of forward progression used. In the present study, focus is placed on trunk movements in the sagittal, coronal, and transverse/rotation plane, as well as placement of the CoM, during gait in children with juvenile idiopathic arthritis (JIA). Seventeen children with JIA, all with polyarticular lower extremity involvement were examined before and approximately two weeks after treatment with intra-articular cortico-steroid injections. Movement was recorded with a 6-camera 3D motion analysis system in both the children with JIA and in 21 healthy controls. Trunk and center of mass movements were compared between JIA and controls, and effects of intra-articular cortico-steroid treatment were evaluated. Children with JIA were more posteriorly tilted in the trunk, contrary to the common clinical impression, and had their CoM placed more posterior and off-centred, which may have been a result of pain. With such knowledge, it might be possible to better understand the effects of their pain and involvement, and ultimately to plan a treatment strategy for improving their gait patterns. 相似文献
137.
A validation study of a computerised test recently developed involving the Stroop effect, extended here by inclusion of a third, more difficult test series, is presented. Three groups of men belonging to the Swedish armed forces and adjudged to differ in their qualifications (20, 32, and 19 men of levels 1, 2, and 3, respectively) and a fourth group of 18 men convicted of serious crimes of violence were given this test, termed the Stress Strategy Test. Discriminant analysis of the test's 12 variables (four for each of the three test series) yielded a discriminant power of 65% for the total group, highest for the level 1 group (80%) and for the nonmilitary group (72%), results substantially better than obtained for the original version of the test with use of similar subject groups. 相似文献
138.
Pär Sundström 《Philosophical Studies》2007,136(2):123-165
Colours and consciousness both present us with metaphysical problems. But what exactly are the problems? According to standard accounts, they are roughly the following. On the one hand, we have reason to believe, about both colour and consciousness, that they are identical with some familiar natural phenomena. But on the other hand, it is hard to see how these identities could obtain. I argue that this is an adequate characterisation of our metaphysical problem of colour, but a mischaracterisation of the problem of consciousness. It mischaracterises the problem by presenting consciousness as more ‘colour-like’ than we have reason to take it to be. The real problem of consciousness is, I suggest, that almost nothing theoretically useful is known about this phenomenon at present. I also explore some implications of this perspective on the problem of consciousness. Given the shape of the problem, I argue that we can’t rule out all forms of eliminativism about consciousness. Nor can we rule out that future research will close the ‘explanatory gap’ that consciousness gives rise to. 相似文献
139.
The present study compared differences between Japanese and Swedish schoolchildren in learning motivation-related variables in physical education. The subjects were 1,562 Japanese fifth and sixth graders (776 boys and 786 girls) ranging in age from 10 to 12 years and 573 Swedish fifth graders (306 boys and 267 girls) from 10 to 13 years (M = 11.4, SD = 0.5). They completed three questionnaires to evaluate the children's learning motivation, factors supporting motivation to learn, and preferences of learning behavior. The questionnaires were taken from Nishida's Diagnosis of Learning Motivation in Physical Education Test, a multidimensional and comprehensive test that measures learning motivation. A 2 x 2 (country by sex) multivariate analysis of variance indicated both Swedish boys and girls scored significantly higher than the Japanese children on most subscales. Results were discussed in relation to differences in the sports environment and culture of the two countries. 相似文献
140.
Aïna Chalabaev Philippe Sarrazin David Trouilloud Lee Jussim 《Journal of applied social psychology》2009,39(10):2469-2498
This research investigated different forms of sex bias in teacher expectations relative to gymnastics performance. First, a laboratory experiment including 163 physical education teachers confirmed that stereotypes favorable to boys may influence teacher expectations in gymnastics. Next, a naturalistic study involving 15 teachers and 422 students showed that teachers expected no sex differences, even though girls performed better than boys. However, this sex bias was a result of reliance on nondiagnostic student personal characteristics favorable to boys, rather than on a stereotype per se. These results suggest that egalitarian beliefs may mask a bias in favor of a social group when group differences actually exist, and that sex‐biased teacher expectations do not inevitably involve an influence of sex stereotypes. 相似文献