全文获取类型
收费全文 | 430篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
434篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 24篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 27篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 58篇 |
2012年 | 22篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 23篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有434条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
The ability to predict the effects of actions is necessary to behave properly in our physical and social world. Here, we describe how the ability to predict the consequence of complex gestures can change the way we integrate sight and sound when relevant visual information is missing. Six drummers and six novices were asked to judge audiovisual synchrony for drumming point-light displays where the visual information was manipulated to eliminate or include the drumstick–drumhead impact point. In the condition with only the arm information novices were unable to detect asynchrony whereas drummers were able to. Additionally, in the conditions that included the impact point drummers perceived the best alignment when the sight preceded the sound, while in the arm only condition they perceived the best alignment when the sound occurred together with or preceded the sight, as it would be expected if they were predicting the beat occurrence. Taken together these findings suggest that humans can acquire, through practice, internal models of action which can be used to replace missing information when integrating multisensory signals from the environment. 相似文献
252.
Amanuel G. Tekleab Karin A. Orvis M. Susan Taylor 《Journal of business and psychology》2013,28(3):361-374
Purpose
This study examines the issue of change in newcomers’ employer-based psychological contract obligations over time, viewing change as a potentially important determinant of perceived contract breach and subsequent employee attitudes and behaviors.Design/Methodology/Approach
Data were collected using a three-wave longitudinal design from newly hired faculty members (N = 106).Findings
Newcomers’ perceptions of employer-based relational obligations significantly decreased during their first year on the job. Newcomers reacted negatively to these changes, subsequently reporting increased contract breach and more negative work attitudes (i.e., increased turnover intentions and reduced job satisfaction and organizational loyalty).Implications
This study provides evidence of the negative effects of perceived changes to a newcomer’s psychological contract. Practitioners should implement interventions to ensure a realistic set of psychological contract obligations are developed from the start in order to minimize the likelihood that newcomers will modify these obligations downward; and, therefore, experience these negative attitudes toward the organization.Originality/Value
Drawing from the realistic job preview and socialization literatures, this study examines a topic that has received little empirical attention in the extant psychological contract research, yet has important implications to the management of employees’ psychological contracts. Using both a three-wave longitudinal field design and a more rigorous statistical analysis for assessing change (i.e., latent growth curve modeling), we add a unique contribution to the extant research by identifying the negative consequences of psychological contract change on newcomers’ subsequent work perceptions and attitudes. 相似文献253.
Dr. med. Dipl.-Psych. Karin Maria Kernhof Alexander Obbarius Johannes Kaufhold Wolfgang Merkle Ralph Grabhorn 《Psychotherapeut》2013,58(2):152-158
Background
Countertransference is seen as an instrument of research into a patient’s unconscious and is therapeutically useful in furthering psychodynamic work. For inpatient psychodynamic psychotherapy, where patients are treated by a team of psychotherapists, countertransference is much more complex and makes consideration and integration of different countertransference feelings necessary. Using the first German translation of the countertransference questionnaire (CTQ, Zittel Conclin and Westen, the countertransference questionnaire, 2005) an attempt was made to identify differences in countertransference phenomena in a team of psychotherapists and to show the impact countertransference has on therapy outcome.Materials and methods
A total of t multiprofessional teams of psychotherapists (15 physicians, 5 psychologists and 9 body and art therapy psychotherapists) took part in the study and completed the CTQ for 137 patients (100 female, 37 male) at the beginning and at the end of inpatient psychotherapy. In addition, the patients completed two questionnaires, the Symptom Check List Revised 90-R (SCL-90) for the severity of symptoms and the Assessment of DSM-IV Personality Disorders Questionnaire (ADP-IV) for personality disorders.Results
Body and art therapy psychotherapists expressed more positive, protective and involved countertransference feelings whereas psychotherapists for single and group therapy expressed more aggressive and hopeless feelings. Countertransference has an impact on therapy outcome and feelings of a lack of interest, aggression and resignation at the beginning of therapy can point to a poor outcome. Patients with personality disorders activate more negative countertransference reactions than patients without personality disorders but at the same time they may also evoke parent-like feelings of protection.Conclusions
Using the CTQ it is possible to differentiate countertransference phenomena in a team of psychotherapists treating inpatients with psychodynamic psychotherapy. The timely perception and integration of countertransference feelings has a positive influence on the therapeutic process and therapy outcome. 相似文献254.
Schweigert WA Cintron J Sullivan K Ilic E Ellis S Dobrowits C Roberts C 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2003,32(4):455-475
In a series of three studies, characteristics of novel figurative phrases were compared with those of established idioms. Studies 1 and 2 found that certain correlations were found to be strong and in one direction for familiar idioms and comparably strong but in the opposite direction for novel figurative phrases, with the correlations for less familiar idioms usually falling partway between these extremes. Study 2 also noted that written or auditory presentation had minimal effects on characteristic ratings. In Study 3 the correlations among characteristics stayed relatively constant for familiar idioms after one, three, or six presentations, but for the novel figurative phrases, after multiple presentations the correlations changed in the direction of the established idioms. 相似文献
255.
This introductory essay provides the context for the articles in this Themed Section. Despite the diversity in locations, historical backgrounds and contemporary processes of change, all contributors to this Themed Section focus on the struggle of Muslim groups over access to an emergent Muslim public sphere. They highlight the contestations of and shifts in the notions of agency, belonging, and citizenship in nation-states with Muslim communities within its borders. The introduction consists of two parts. The first part reviews the notion of the public sphere as conceptualized by Habermas and critiqued by scholars of a diversity of backgrounds. In relation to the concept of the Muslim public sphere, three aspects of critique are given closer consideration in this first part: the value of thinking in terms of multiple publics, the loss of legitimacy of traditional religious authorities, and the importance of agency and identity that allow individuals to engage in a diversity of publics. The second part introduces the various contributions in the Themed Section. 相似文献
256.
Kristina N. Bauer Karin A. Orvis Katherine Ely Eric A. Surface 《Journal of business and psychology》2016,31(1):33-50
Purpose
The extant research has not been consistent in the way motivation is conceptualized and measured in learning contexts, with prior research utilizing five different types of motivation derived from three theoretical frameworks—self-determination theory, expectancy theory, and the expectancy-value model. The purpose of the present study was to examine whether type of motivation impacts the motivation–training outcome relationships.Design/Methodology
We conducted a meta-analysis investigating the impact of motivation type (i.e., intrinsic motivation, motivation to learn, motivation to transfer, expectancy motivation, and task value) on four training outcomes. The review of the literature yielded 136 independent samples and a total of 25,012 trainees. Relative weights analysis was also used.Findings
Results suggest that all types of motivation had stronger relationships with trainee reactions than with declarative knowledge, initial skill acquisition, or transfer. Yet, there was variability in the strength of the motivation–training outcome relationships across motivation type.Implications
We recommend that motivation to learn be used to predict trainee reactions, declarative knowledge, and initial skill acquisition; motivation to transfer should be measured when predicting distal post-training outcomes (i.e., transfer of training). Although this recommendation may seem intuitive, clearly prior research/practice has used other motivation types in the prediction of these training outcomes. Accordingly, we advise that measures of motivation to learn and motivation to transfer be used more uniformly.Originality/Value
This is the first study to meta-analytically test whether the relationship between motivation and training outcomes varies based on the type of motivation utilized.257.
Afsaneh Hayat Roshanai Peter Lindgren Karin Nordin Charlotta Ingvoldstad 《Journal of genetic counseling》2016,25(1):146-156
The aim of this study was to investigate the opinions of Swedish university students about information regarding soft markers, when observed at second trimester ultrasound screening. A cross-sectional survey, where 85 Swedish university students completed a study specific questionnaire containing eleven hypothetical scenarios, involving various parameters (disease/syndromes/malformations with different characteristics), and location of the markers. Almost all participants indicated that they would wish to be informed, prior to the ultrasound examination, about the assessment and significance of soft markers. However, the number of respondents who requested information about a potential actual finding was considerably less. Several participants wanted to be informed about detected markers associated to serious conditions but not when the marker indicated an increased risk for a treatable disease. Also, the specific location of the marker was of importance to the participants. The majority of respondents wished to be informed about the soft markers if they were observed in the heart or the brain of the foetus, compared to if they were located in the intestine or the skeleton. The students’ opinion, in this study, implicate the importance of pre-examination information to enable expectant parents to make informed choice regarding the second trimester ultrasound screening as well as on reciving information of actual findings. 相似文献
258.
Karin C. Brocki Lilianne Eninger Lisa B. Thorell Gunilla Bohlin 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2010,38(2):163-171
The present study, including children at risk for developing Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), examined the
idea that complex executive functions (EFs) build upon more simple ones. This notion was applied in the study of longitudinal
interrelations between core EF components — simple and complex inhibition, selective attention, and working memory (WM) —
at age 5 and 6 as well as their predictive relations to ADHD symptoms at age 7. The results showed that simple inhibition
and selective attention at age 5 independently predicted complex inhibition and WM at age 6. In addition, EFs primarily predicted
symptoms of inattention rather than hyperactivity/impulsivity even at this young age. Finally, age 6 complex inhibition was
shown to act as a mediator in the relations between simple inhibition and selective attention at age 5 and symptoms of inattention
at age 7. These findings provide novel longitudinal support for the theory that fundamental EF components show a progression
with age toward more complex executive control (see Garon et al. Psychological Bulletin 134(1):31–60 2008). Further, complex inhibition, implicating both inhibition and WM, seems to be a particularly strong correlate of ADHD symptoms
in young children and should as such be the focus of future studies examining the relation between cognitive function and
ADHD symptoms from a developmental perspective. 相似文献
259.
IBM PCs and compatibles are increasingly used in psychological research for tachistoscopic tasks, which require precise control of timing of stimulus durations and accurate measurement of response latencies. The application of MEL in such research is discussed; several precautions which influence stimulus timing are noted, and some data on measurement error with keypress responses are presented. 相似文献
260.
Jerry M. Lewis J. M. Rabbie Thomas Berg Michael Argyle Karin Aronsson Roger Ingham Neil Frude A. M. Munton Duncan Cramer John Shotter Ray Holland Nobleza Asuncion-Lande Ray Crozier Candida Peterson Chris Spencer Victor J. Callan Jack Martin N. L. Reinsch Anthony Ryle David Canter Jill Robson H. R. Beech Duncan Moss David Fontana Tom Richards 《Current Psychology》1989,8(1):49-87