首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   73122篇
  免费   3387篇
  国内免费   68篇
  2020年   788篇
  2019年   945篇
  2018年   1368篇
  2017年   1408篇
  2016年   1458篇
  2015年   963篇
  2014年   1202篇
  2013年   6120篇
  2012年   2226篇
  2011年   2444篇
  2010年   1496篇
  2009年   1572篇
  2008年   2170篇
  2007年   2184篇
  2006年   1892篇
  2005年   1730篇
  2004年   1708篇
  2003年   1577篇
  2002年   1632篇
  2001年   2378篇
  2000年   2242篇
  1999年   1737篇
  1998年   946篇
  1997年   810篇
  1996年   764篇
  1995年   711篇
  1994年   715篇
  1993年   731篇
  1992年   1425篇
  1991年   1344篇
  1990年   1353篇
  1989年   1238篇
  1988年   1172篇
  1987年   1166篇
  1986年   1156篇
  1985年   1225篇
  1984年   964篇
  1983年   846篇
  1982年   707篇
  1979年   998篇
  1978年   746篇
  1975年   905篇
  1974年   951篇
  1973年   928篇
  1972年   847篇
  1971年   752篇
  1969年   722篇
  1968年   882篇
  1967年   843篇
  1966年   786篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
The Evaluative Space Model of emotions allows for the coactivation of positive‐appetitive and negative‐avoidant systems, but few studies have examined mixed emotions in child development. Existing research suggests children's understanding of opposite valence emotion combinations emerges by approximately 11 years of age. However, it is not yet clear whether various opposite valence combinations are understood at different ages, nor whether children can understand them in others before they have experienced such mixed emotions themselves. Semi‐structured interviews with 97 children investigated whether they regarded six combinations of opposite valence mixed emotions as possible, could provide reasons for them, and report their own experience of each in the context of mother–child relationships. Both understanding that such combinations are possible and ability to provide reasons for them increased after age 6 and up to age 11, but were still incomplete in 12‐year‐olds. Understanding of different opposite valence combinations developed at different rates. At each age, fewer children who showed understanding of these combinations in others reported having had a similar experience themselves. The findings suggest a need to systematically examine a range of mixed emotions in order to develop a comprehensive theory of the development of mixed emotion understanding. They also suggest extending research into adolescence.  相似文献   
42.
This study evaluated cardiovascular responses (CVR) to an active speech task with blatantly discriminatory (BRC) versus neutral (NRC) stimuli and an anger recall task in a sample of Black men (N = 73; age 18 to 47). Diastolic blood pressure scores were higher for NRC versus BRC stimuli during anger recall (p = .05). Moreover, persons in the NRC group who perceived high levels of racism (vs. no racism or BRC group) during active speech showed larger increases in blood pressure across postspeech rest, anger recall, and subsequent rest (p = .03). The notable elevation in CVR in response to an ambiguous event extends current models of racism suggesting that subtle racism is a psychosocial stressor that erodes health through chronically elevated CVR.  相似文献   
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号