首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   446篇
  免费   30篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   13篇
  2021年   18篇
  2020年   21篇
  2019年   27篇
  2018年   32篇
  2017年   29篇
  2016年   34篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   38篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   8篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   4篇
  2006年   5篇
  1999年   3篇
  1997年   7篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   3篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   3篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   5篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   5篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   6篇
  1973年   9篇
  1972年   3篇
  1971年   8篇
  1969年   10篇
  1968年   7篇
  1967年   5篇
  1965年   3篇
  1964年   4篇
  1963年   3篇
  1962年   3篇
  1961年   6篇
  1960年   5篇
  1958年   12篇
  1956年   3篇
  1955年   6篇
  1953年   6篇
排序方式: 共有476条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
The purpose of this research is to quantitatively compare everyday situational experience around the world. Local collaborators recruited 5,447 members of college communities in 20 countries, who provided data via a Web site in 14 languages. Using the 89 items of the Riverside Situational Q‐sort (RSQ), participants described the situation they experienced the previous evening at 7:00 p.m. Correlations among the average situational profiles of each country ranged from r = .73 to r = .95; the typical situation was described as largely pleasant. Most similar were the United States/Canada; least similar were South Korea/Denmark. Japan had the most homogenous situational experience; South Korea, the least. The 15 RSQ items varying the most across countries described relatively negative aspects of situational experience; the 15 least varying items were more positive. Further analyses correlated RSQ items with national scores on six value dimensions, the Big Five traits, economic output, and population. Individualism, Neuroticism, Openness, and Gross Domestic Product yielded more significant correlations than expected by chance. Psychological research traditionally has paid more attention to the assessment of persons than of situations, a discrepancy that extends to cross‐cultural psychology. The present study demonstrates how cultures vary in situational experience in psychologically meaningful ways.  相似文献   
132.
133.
Some prior research indicated that self-image threat may lead people to stereotyping and prejudiced evaluations of others. Other studies found that self-image threat may promote less stereotypical thinking and unprejudiced behavior. In a series of three studies, we demonstrate that self-image threat may lead to either more or less stereotypical perception of the outgroup depending on the level of the individuals` motivation toward closure (NFC). The results reveal that when individuals high (vs. low) in NFC perceived a member of an outgroup, they are less likely to use stereotypical traits if their self-image had been threatened by negative feedback (Study 1) or if they had imagined an example of their own immoral activity (Studies 2 and 3). Moreover, our results demonstrate that the fear of invalidity resulting from self-image threat induction is responsible for the foregoing effects (Study 3). These results are discussed in light of theories of motivational readiness and lay epistemics.  相似文献   
134.
135.
Moral dilemmas often force us to decide between deontological (harming others is wrong) and utilitarian (harming others can be acceptable depending on the consequences) considerations. Cognitive scientists have shown that utilitarian responders typically engage demanding deliberate thinking to override a conflicting intuitive deontological response. A key question is whether deontic responders also take utilitarian considerations into account and detect that there are conflicting responses at play. The present study addressed this issue by contrasting people's processing of moral dilemmas in which utilitarian and deontological considerations cued conflicting or non-conflicting decisions. Results showed that deontic responders were slower and less confident about their decision when solving the conflict (vs. no-conflict) dilemmas. This suggests that they are considering both deontic and utilitarian aspects of their decision and indicates that a deontic decision is more informed and less oblivious than it might appear.  相似文献   
136.
Różycka-Tran  Joanna  Piotrowski  Jarosław P.  Żemojtel-Piotrowska  Magdalena  Jurek  Paweł  Osin  Evgeny N.  Adams  Byron G.  Ardi  Rahkman  Bălțătescu  Sergiu  Bhomi  Arbinda Lal  Bogomaz  Sergey A.  Cieciuch  Jan  Clinton  Amanda  de Clunie  Gisela T.  Czarna  Anna Z.  Esteves  Carla Sofia  Gouveia  Valdiney  Halik  Murnizam H. J.  Kachatryan  Narine  Kamble  Shanmukh Vasant  Kawula  Anna  Klicperova-Baker  Martina  Kospakov  Aituar  Letovancova  Eva  Lun  Vivian Miu-Chi  Cerrato  Sara Malo  Muehlbacher  Stephan  Nikolic  Marija  Pankratova  Alina A.  Park  Joonha  Paspalanova  Elena  Pék  Győző  de León  Pablo Pérez  Šolcová  Iva Poláčková  Shahbaz  Wahab  Ha  Truong Thi Khanh  Tiliouine  Habib  Van Hiel  Alain  Vauclair  Christin-Melanie  Wills-Herrera  Eduardo  Włodarczyk  Anna  Yahiiaiev  Illia I.  Maltby  John 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2021,40(7):3575-3584
Current Psychology - This article presents a short research report on the relationship between perceived antagonism in social relations measured using the Belief in a Zero-Sum Game (BZSG) scale,...  相似文献   
137.
138.
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences - It is a well-known fact that mathematics plays a crucial role in physics; in fact, it is virtually impossible to imagine contemporary physics without it....  相似文献   
139.
Journal of Religion and Health - This article argues that Christianity has the potential to strengthen people’s health when solving the forgiveness dilemma....  相似文献   
140.
Topoi - Gestural and pantomimic accounts of language origins propose that language did not develop directly from ape vocalisations, but rather that its emergence was preceded by an intervening...  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号