首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   412篇
  免费   18篇
  2023年   5篇
  2022年   4篇
  2021年   18篇
  2020年   16篇
  2019年   23篇
  2018年   25篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   21篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   15篇
  2013年   41篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   20篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   13篇
  2007年   16篇
  2006年   16篇
  2005年   12篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   8篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   5篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   5篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   2篇
  1977年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
  1973年   2篇
  1971年   1篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   2篇
排序方式: 共有430条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
421.
422.
423.
Editorial     
  相似文献   
424.
425.
When a woman says "I am a feminist" what does she mean? What do other people think she means? We asked 71 women to complete the Feminist Perspectives Scale (Henley, Meng, O'Brien, McCarthy, & Sockloskie, 1998) from their own perspective and from the perspective of a "typical feminist." Women who self-identified as feminists had stronger beliefs than those who did not on all feminist perspectives except cultural feminism. Both groups believed that a typical feminist held stronger radical, socialist, and cultural feminist beliefs than they themselves did, although the discrepancies were greater for nonfeminists. Nonfeminists viewed a typical feminist as endorsing stronger cultural feminist views than did feminists. Our results indicate that feminist self-identity is related to endorsement of feminist ideologies, and that both feminists and nonfeminists think that a typical feminist is more extreme than they are. The results also suggest that cultural feminism is a contested ideology; it is not endorsed by feminists, but is ascribed to them by nonfeminists  相似文献   
426.
427.
Bodian  Miriam 《Jewish History》2021,35(3-4):329-349
Jewish History - For more than a century, scholars of early modern Spanish heterodoxy accepted the Inquisition’s categories of heresy as their own. The various types of heretics the...  相似文献   
428.
429.
ABSTRACT

Perceived superiority, the tendency to regard one’s own relationship as better than other people’s relationships, is a key relationship maintenance mechanism. Little is known about whether and how it changes during the transition to marriage, a pivotal moment in most couples’ life cycle. In a longitudinal study following 97 couples for three waves across the transition, men presented stable perceived superiority, whereas women presented a curvilinear change in superiority perceptions, with a substantial increase in perceived superiority between T1 and T2 and a significantly reduced change between T2 and T3. In addition, trajectories differed according to partners’ commitment level. More committed and less committed partners both showed a curvilinear change in perceived superiority, though following different patterns. Results point to the functional value of perceived superiority, which emerges as a strategy aimed at sustaining partners through the challenges deriving from the transition to marriage.  相似文献   
430.
ABSTRACT

Political and social changes in the past decade have rendered questions about religion and immigration more salient than ever. However, we know very little about the potential impact of religion as it operates in the real world on attitudes toward immigrants. In this investigation, we tested whether and how contextual religious cues in the public sphere might affect tolerance toward immigrants. In two studies, we compared the effects of a religious and a secular context (Study 1: religious location; Study 2: religious attire) on attitudes toward Jewish immigrants (i.e., a religious ingroup) and non-Jewish immigrants (i.e., a religious outgroup). Across studies, contextual religious cues predicted ingroup favoritism, as expressed by less social rejection toward religious ingroups and less support for anti-immigration policies affecting religious ingroups. However, contextual religious cues were unrelated to anti-immigration attitudes toward religious outgroups. In Study 2, these patterns were moderated by participants’ religiosity, such that they were found among more (but not fewer) religious participants. These findings extend prior laboratory findings and shed light on how religion influences attitudes toward immigration in rich and complex real environments.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号