全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6268篇 |
免费 | 297篇 |
专业分类
6565篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 15篇 |
2023年 | 22篇 |
2022年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 116篇 |
2020年 | 165篇 |
2019年 | 480篇 |
2018年 | 354篇 |
2017年 | 447篇 |
2016年 | 445篇 |
2015年 | 408篇 |
2014年 | 396篇 |
2013年 | 715篇 |
2012年 | 393篇 |
2011年 | 304篇 |
2010年 | 322篇 |
2009年 | 292篇 |
2008年 | 269篇 |
2007年 | 176篇 |
2006年 | 179篇 |
2005年 | 177篇 |
2004年 | 133篇 |
2003年 | 134篇 |
2002年 | 140篇 |
2001年 | 101篇 |
2000年 | 76篇 |
1999年 | 45篇 |
1998年 | 18篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有6565条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
James W. Beck Abigail A. Scholer Aaron M. Schmidt 《Journal of business and psychology》2017,32(4):421-440
Purpose
Shortcut behaviors are methods of completing a task that require less time than typical or standard procedures. These behaviors carry the benefit of increasing efficiency, yet can also carry risks (e.g., of an accident). The purpose of this research is to understand the reasons individuals engage in shortcut behaviors, even when doing so is known to be risky.Design/Methodology/Approach
We present two laboratory studies (N = 121 and N = 144) in which participants performed an air traffic control simulation. Participants could improve efficiency by taking shortcuts; that is, by sending aircraft off the prescribed flight paths. This design allowed for direct and unobtrusive observation of shortcut behaviors.Findings
Individuals who were told that efficiency was an obligation tended to believe that shortcut behaviors had utility for managing high workloads, even when the risks associated with shortcuts were high. Downstream, utility perceptions were positively related to actual shortcut behavior.Implications
Although communicating risks may be used to help individuals balance the “pros” and “cons” of shortcut behaviors, goal framing is also important. Subtle cues indicating that efficiency is an obligation can lead to elevated perceptions of the utility of shortcut behaviors, even when knowing that engaging in shortcut behaviors is very risky.Originality/Value
Past research has provided limited insights into the reasons individuals sometimes engage in shortcut behaviors even when doing so is known to be risky. The current research speaks to this issue by identifying workload and obligation framing as antecedents of the decision to take shortcuts.52.
Chieh‐Chen Bowen Janet K. Swim Rick R. Jacobs 《Journal of applied social psychology》2000,30(10):2194-2215
This study examined gender bias on job performance in work settings where confounding variables (e. g., organizational level, experience, education) were cautiously taken into consideration to ensure fair comparisons. Although previous meta‐analyses examined gender biases on evaluations, findings in tightly controlled laboratory environments may differ from those in highly complicated field studies. We found little evidence of overall gender bias in performance appraisals in nonconfounded field studies. However, there were significant pro‐male biases when only men served as raters. Measure‐specific gender stereotypicality, instead of genera! stereotypicality about the job, produced gender bias in performance appraisal. Masculine measures produced pro‐male bias, and feminine measures produced pro‐female bias. 相似文献
53.
54.
Michael A. Busseri Linda Rose‐Krasnor 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2009,27(4):907-933
In recent years, an impressive volume of evidence has accumulated demonstrating that youth involvement in structured, organized activities (e.g. school sports, community clubs) may facilitate positive youth development. We present a theory‐based framework for studying structured activity involvement (SAI) as a context for positive youth development based on two key dimensions: breadth and intensity of involvement. Our main goal is to demonstrate the separability, salience, and developmental significance of these two dimensions. We review three developmental theoretical approaches (identity development, life‐span selection–optimization–compensation theory, and affordances) that support our conceptualization of breadth and intensity as salient and significant dimensions of SAI. We also summarize our recent program of research on SAI demonstrating the separability of breadth and intensity dimensions, which shows links between these dimensions and indicators of positive development. Finally, we discuss how the proposed breadth‐intensity approach could be used to extend research on the linkage between youth SAI and successful development. 相似文献
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
Moral foundations theory has recently emerged from the field of social psychology as a means for understanding cultural, political, and religious differences between individuals and groups. The authors review the moral foundations theory literature to help counselors in understanding the roots of their clients' and their own moral belief systems and how these systems contribute to decision-making pertaining to morally laden topics. Lastly, the authors present a model for self-awareness around moral issues along with a fictional case study intended to illustrate the use of the model. 相似文献
60.
Ben Jones 《Journal of applied philosophy》2020,37(2):233-248
Those who care about and engage in politics frequently fall victim to cognitive bias. Concerns that such bias impacts scholarship recently have prompted debates – notably, in philosophy and psychology – on the proper relationship between research and politics. One proposal emerging from these debates is that researchers studying politics have a professional duty to avoid political activism because it risks biasing their work. While sympathetic to the motivations behind this proposal, I suggest several reasons to reject a blanket duty to avoid activism: (1) even if it reduced bias, this duty would make unreasonable demands on researchers; (2) this duty could hinder research by limiting viewpoint diversity; (3) this duty wrongly implies that academia offers a relative haven from bias compared to politics; and (4) not all forms of political activism pose an equal risk of bias. None of these points suggest that researchers should ignore the risk of bias. Rather, researchers should focus on stronger evidence-based strategies for reducing bias than a blanket recommendation to avoid politics. 相似文献