全文获取类型
收费全文 | 142篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
148篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 5篇 |
2016年 | 2篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 10篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 7篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 3篇 |
2008年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 6篇 |
2006年 | 3篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1963年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有148条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
The existence of a phonemic deficit that is predictive of, and probably causal to, many cases of reading difficulty is well established. Tallal (1984) has suggested that this phonemic deficit is in fact a symptom of an underlying auditory temporal processing deficit. Our purpose in this paper is to evaluate the plausibility of this hypothesis. The various components that might constitute sequential (or temporal) processing are described. Our review of the literature reveals considerable evidence for a deficit in dyslexics in stimulus individuation tasks (e.g., gap detection) and temporal order judgments in both the auditory and visual modalities. The possibility that a general temporal processing deficit is associated with dyslexia, as suggested by Tallal (1984), is explored, and possible etiologies for such a deficit are discussed. The possibility of a causal link between temporal processing deficits and some reading disabilities is demonstrated, and converging evidence from morphological studies is reviewed. It is concluded that a temporal processing deficit does appear to be present in many developmental dyslexics, and strategies are suggested for further research aimed at evaluating the hypothesis that this deficit may be the root cause of a number of cases of dyslexia itself. 相似文献
132.
Heterogeneity of popular boys: antisocial and prosocial configurations 总被引:21,自引:0,他引:21
This study examined subtypes of popular 4th-6th grade boys (N = 452). Popular-prosocial (model) and popular-antisocial (tough) configurations were identified by means of teacher ratings and compared with peer and self-assessments and social centrality measures. Peers perceived model boys as cool, athletic, leaders, cooperative, studious, not shy, and nonaggressive. Peers perceived tough boys as cool, athletic, and antisocial. Model boys saw themselves as nonaggressive and academically competent. Tough boys saw themselves as popular, aggressive, and physically competent. Tough boys were disproportionately African American, particularly when African Americans were a minority in their classrooms. Model and tough boys were overrepresented at nuclear social centrality levels. These findings suggest that highly aggressive boys can be among the most popular and socially connected children in elementary classrooms. 相似文献
133.
McSweeney FK Farmer VA Dougan JD Whipple JE 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1986,45(1):83-101
The literature was examined to determine how well the generalized matching law (Baum, 1974) describes multiple-schedule responding. In general, it describes the data well, accounting for a median of 91% of the variance. The median size of the undermatching parameter was 0.46; the median bias parameter was 1.00. The size of the undermatching parameter, and the proportion of the variance accounted for by the equation, varied inversely with the number of schedules conducted, with the number of sessions conducted per schedule, and with the time within a component. The undermatching parameter also varied with the operanda used to produce reinforcers and with the reinforcer used. The undermatching parameter did not vary consistently with component duration or with several other variables. Bias was greater when fewer rather than more schedules were conducted, when two rather than one operanda were used, and when White Carneaux rather than homing pigeons served as subjects. These results imply that the generalized matching law may describe both concurrent and multiple-schedule responding, but that the same variables do not always influence the bias and undermatching parameters in the same way for the two types of schedules. 相似文献
134.
Exercise Stage of Change and Self-Efficacy in Primary Care: Implications for Intervention 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Rita Cowan Everett Logue Lori Milo Paula J. Britton William Smucker 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》1997,4(3):295-311
Multiple chronic diseases are caused or complicated by a sedentary lifestyle. Thus, an important and challenging application of psychology in clinical settings is changing the behavior of sedentary primary care patients. This study focused on exercise stage of change and self-efficacy in a sample of adult family practice patients recruited while waiting for their scheduled physician appointment. Regarding exercise stage of change, 15% of respondents were in the Precontemplation stage, 26% in the Contemplation stage, 50% in the Preparation stage, 7% in the Action stage, and 13% in the Maintenance stage. Mean self-efficacy scores for exercise were significantly higher among respondents in the Action and Maintenance stages of change. These cross-sectional data are consistent with the hypothesis that movement through the exercise stages of change could be encouraged by clinical interventions that increase exercise self-efficacy. The identification of multiple personal opportunities for increasing exercise self-efficacy may be clinically useful in this context. Recommendations for psychologists in primary care settings in their work with physicians are offered. 相似文献
135.
William M. Farmer 《Erkenntnis》1995,43(3):279-294
Partial functions are ubiquitous in both mathematics and computer science. Therefore, it is imperative that the underlying logical formalism for a general-purpose mechanized mathematics system provide strong support for reasoning about partial functions. Unfortunately, the common logical formalisms — first-order logic, type theory, and set theory — are usually only adequate for reasoning about partial functionsin theory. However, the approach to partial functions traditionally employed by mathematicians is quite adequatein practice. This paper shows how the traditional approach to partial functions can be formalized in a range of formalisms that includes first-order logic, simple type theory, and Von-Neumann—Bernays—Gödel set theory. It argues that these new formalisms allow one to directly reason about partial functions; are based on natural, well-understood, familiar principles; and can be effectively implemented in mechanized mathematics systems.Supported by the MITRE-Sponsored Research program. This paper is a written version (with references) of an address given at the Partial Functions and Programming: Foundational Questions conference held 17 February 1995 at the University ol California at Irvine. 相似文献
136.
Early adolescent age and gender differences in patterns of emotional self-disclosure to parents and friends 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
This study explored adolescent age and gender differences in patterns of emotional self-disclosure to parents and friends. The sample consisted of 174 junior high school students between the ages of 12 and 15. Results revealed that females exhibited greater emotional self-disclosure to parents and peers than did males, and that emotional self-disclosure to friends was greatest among older adolescents. In addition, while younger adolescents preferred to disclose information about their emotional state to parents, older adolescents chose friends. Exploratory hierarchical regression analyses revealed that emotional disclosure to parents was most strongly associated with adolescent perceptions of the openness of family communication, family cohesion, and satisfaction with family relationships. Emotional disclosure to friends was associated with adolescent self-esteem in the peer context and identity development. The results are discussed in terms of the complementary socializing processes that may operate within the contexts of family and friends. 相似文献
137.
David B. Estell Thomas W. Farmer Matthew J. Irvin Amity Crowther Patrick Akos Daniel J. Boudah 《Journal of child and family studies》2009,18(2):136-150
We examined bullying and victimization in 5th grade classrooms in relation to students’ education status and peer group membership.
The sample consisted of 484 participants (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369 general education students, 74 academically
gifted students, and 41 students with mild disabilities. Students with mild disabilities were more likely to be perceived
as being bullies by both teachers and peers. Teachers also rated students with mild disabilities significantly higher for
being bullied by peers. Academically gifted students were rated by teachers as the lowest for both bullying and being bullied.
Associating with aggressive or perceived-popular peers increased the likelihood of being perceived as a bully. Social isolates
were more likely to be bullied than students who did not associate with perceived-popular peers who, in turn, were more likely
to be bullied than students who associated with perceived-popular peers. Students with mild disabilities who had aggressive
and perceived-popular associates had more peer nominations for bullying than all others. In contrast, students in general
education with neither aggressive nor perceived-popular associates had the fewest peer nominations for bullying. We discuss
implications for research and intervention. 相似文献
138.
The seven virtues of simple type theory 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Simple type theory, also known as higher-order logic, is a natural extension of first-order logic which is simple, elegant, highly expressive, and practical. This paper surveys the virtues of simple type theory and attempts to show that simple type theory is an attractive alternative to first-order logic for practical-minded scientists, engineers, and mathematicians. It recommends that simple type theory be incorporated into introductory logic courses offered by mathematics departments and into the undergraduate curricula for computer science and software engineering students. 相似文献
139.
Several theories have been proposed to account for the apparent non-responsiveness to punishment cues or aversive events demonstrated by members of some disinhibited groups. Included among these theories are those that emphasize individual differences in temperament, temperament-related biases associated with the allocation of attentional resources, and impairments in executive functions. This study examined the relative contribution of each of these variables to the prediction of passive avoidance errors (PAEs, or failures to inhibit responding to punishment cues) during a computerized go/no-go task. Variations in temperament, attentional allocation to punishment feedback, and executive functions were found to independently and additively contribute to the prediction of PAEs in a mixed sample of men and women recruited at a university campus (n=145). Results from this study, therefore, support multiple theoretical perspectives on PAEs as assessed by the go/no-go experimental paradigm. 相似文献
140.
Donald Fedor John Maslyn Steven Farmer Kenneth Bettenhausen 《Journal of applied social psychology》2008,38(1):76-96
This study investigates whether perceptions of positive (i.e., beneficial) political behaviors are distinct from those of negative political behaviors and the extent to which positive politics perceptions contribute to the prediction of organizationally relevant employee reactions. Data were drawn from 119 survey respondents. The results indicate that, rather than 2 ends of a continuum of political behavior, positive and negative politics represent separate perceptions both of which are seen to occur for individual, group, and organizational foci. In addition, perceptions of positive politics contribute significantly beyond perceptions of negative politics to the prediction of the 4 outcomes used in this study (satisfaction with one's job, supervision, and coworkers; and fulfillment of one's psychological contract with the organization). 相似文献