全文获取类型
收费全文 | 41290篇 |
免费 | 649篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
41942篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 471篇 |
2019年 | 594篇 |
2018年 | 783篇 |
2017年 | 843篇 |
2016年 | 871篇 |
2015年 | 601篇 |
2014年 | 732篇 |
2013年 | 3182篇 |
2012年 | 1342篇 |
2011年 | 1348篇 |
2010年 | 887篇 |
2009年 | 828篇 |
2008年 | 1209篇 |
2007年 | 1224篇 |
2006年 | 1089篇 |
2005年 | 977篇 |
2004年 | 905篇 |
2003年 | 871篇 |
2002年 | 880篇 |
2001年 | 1220篇 |
2000年 | 1165篇 |
1999年 | 904篇 |
1998年 | 429篇 |
1997年 | 404篇 |
1996年 | 371篇 |
1995年 | 382篇 |
1992年 | 760篇 |
1991年 | 694篇 |
1990年 | 742篇 |
1989年 | 639篇 |
1988年 | 666篇 |
1987年 | 632篇 |
1986年 | 631篇 |
1985年 | 583篇 |
1984年 | 528篇 |
1983年 | 512篇 |
1982年 | 384篇 |
1981年 | 374篇 |
1979年 | 595篇 |
1978年 | 435篇 |
1975年 | 480篇 |
1974年 | 526篇 |
1973年 | 560篇 |
1972年 | 429篇 |
1971年 | 416篇 |
1970年 | 370篇 |
1969年 | 419篇 |
1968年 | 493篇 |
1967年 | 454篇 |
1966年 | 381篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
A revised conception of the relationship between psychological science and professional practice is proposed in the light of postmodern changes in perspectives on knowledge. Positivistic science, which has dominated the traditional interpretation of scientist-practitioner training, is considered from a constructivist point of view to be only one possible foundation of psychological knowledge. It is argued that the knowledge base of the profession should be derived with diverse methods and from multiple sources, including the knowledge of practice. The mutuality of science and practice is emphasized. An evolving framework for understanding the epistemology of practice, based on cognitive psychology, is presented. Emphasis on broadened choices of research methods, the development of reflective skills, and better linkage between teaching in the domains of research and practice are urged. Suggestions for research related to scientific training and the knowledge processes of practice are offered. 相似文献
82.
Samuel L. Odom Lynette K. Chandler Michaelene Ostrosky Scott R. McConnell Susan Reaney 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1992,25(2):307-317
This study examined a system for fading teacher prompts to children who served as peers in peer-initiation interventions for young children with disabilities. A teacher taught peers to direct social initiations to children with disabilities, provided verbal prompts for those initiations, and introduced a system that provided peers with visual feedback about the social interactions of the children with disabilities. She then systematically withdrew the verbal prompts to peers, and subsequently faded the visual feedback system. Peer initiations increased when the intervention began and resulted in increases in social interaction for the children with disabilities. As the teacher systematically faded the prompts and visual feedback to the peers, social interaction continued at the levels found during intervention and was maintained during a short maintenance period. 相似文献
83.
S Streufert R M Pogash J Roache D Gingrich R Landis W Severs L Lonardi A Kantner 《The Journal of applied psychology》1992,77(4):515-524
Adult men (N = 44) participated for 2 days (alcohol vs. placebo treatment) in a double-blind, crossover experiment. Performance on the Digit Symbol Substitution Task (DSST) and a visuomotor (VM) task was measured 4 times each day. On the alcohol-treatment day, data were obtained once during ascending breath alcohol levels (BALs), once during maximal BALs (0.05 or 0.10), and twice during descending BALs. Data were collected at the same time points on the placebo-treatment day. Limited evidence for acute tolerance was obtained with the DSST, but error rates on the VM task were higher during maximal and descending BALs. Error rates remained near placebo values, and participants displayed slightly greater caution, while BALs were ascending. Strategy scores on the VM task exceeded placebo scores during maximal intoxication. Data interpretation is focused on individuals in higher level (e.g., professional) positions. 相似文献
84.
Two studies investigated effects of video-assisted training on employment-related social skills of adults with severe mental retardation. In video-assisted training, participants discriminated a model's behavior on videotape and received feedback from the trainer for responses to questions about video scenes. In the first study, 3 adults in an employment program participated in video-assisted training to request their supervisor's assistance when encountering work problems. Results indicated that participants discriminated the target behavior on video but effects did not generalize to the work setting for 2 participants until they rehearsed the behavior. In the second study, 2 participants were taught to fix and report four work problems using video-assisted procedures. Results indicated that after participants rehearsed how to fix and report one or two work problems, they began to fix and report the remaining problems with video-assisted training alone. 相似文献
85.
86.
The classical visual latency explanation of the Pulfrich phenomenon is generalized to allow for the possibility that the visual system uses velocity information from motion detectors to compensate for spatial lag due to time lag, in identifying the instantaneous position of a moving object. The results of the reported experiment verify one consequence of the generalized version: the elliptical apparent path is skewed away from the filtered eye. The results suggest also that the extrapolation factor increases with visual latency and that there is a different extrapolation factor for each eye when the eyes are illuminated unequally. 相似文献
87.
This investigation was conducted to examine whether older adults would improve their performance when given statements intended to make them feel important to a research study as well as statements concerning their over-all well-being. Women (n = 14) and men (n = 15) with mean ages of 76.0 yr. and 75.1 yr., respectively, were randomly assigned into a control group, a positive reinforcement group, and a group given statements of personal importance. Dependent measures on a rotary pursuit task, screw-and-nut assembly task, and an interest questionnaire were obtained during three separate visits. Data were analyzed with a 3 x 3 (Groups x Days) analysis of variance. While no significant group effects were found on any measure, a significant main effect was obtained for days, indicative of learning. 相似文献
88.
This paper suggests a method to supplant missing categorical data by reasonable replacements. These replacements will maximize the consistency of the completed data as measured by Guttman's squared correlation ratio. The text outlines a solution of the optimization problem, describes relationships with the relevant psychometric theory, and studies some properties of the method in detail. The main result is that the average correlation should be at least 0.50 before the method becomes practical. At that point, the technique gives reasonable results up to 10–15% missing data.We thank Anneke Bloemhoff of NIPG-TNO for compiling and making the Dutch Life Style Survey data available to use, and Chantal Houée and Thérèse Bardaine, IUT, Vannes, France, exchange students under the COMETT program of the EC, for computational assistance. We also thank Donald Rubin, the Editors and several anonymous reviewers for constructive suggestions. 相似文献
89.
90.
B P McCloskey R L Klatzky J W Pellegrino 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1992,18(4):948-961
Previous research (Klatzky et al., 1989) shows that the time required to make sensible/nonsensible judgments about an action-object phrase (e.g., "rub your stomach") is reliably faster when the phrase is preceded by a cure representing a specific prototypical hand shape (vs. a neutral cue). The current experiments investigated the effects of preparing for an alternate task (finger tapping vs. syllable vocalization) on facilitatory priming of sensibility judgments. Preparation for finger tapping reduced the magnitude of the priming effect more than preparation for vocalization, suggesting that resources accessed during semantic processing of action-object phrases are also used during manual response preparation. The results support the existence of a system representing manual actions that is limited in the number of activities that can be represented at one time and that is not so general that it represents manual and vocal tract movements. 相似文献