全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6388篇 |
免费 | 1520篇 |
专业分类
7908篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 42篇 |
2022年 | 65篇 |
2021年 | 160篇 |
2020年 | 199篇 |
2019年 | 488篇 |
2018年 | 423篇 |
2017年 | 537篇 |
2016年 | 511篇 |
2015年 | 426篇 |
2014年 | 403篇 |
2013年 | 916篇 |
2012年 | 503篇 |
2011年 | 446篇 |
2010年 | 385篇 |
2009年 | 280篇 |
2008年 | 396篇 |
2007年 | 273篇 |
2006年 | 243篇 |
2005年 | 191篇 |
2004年 | 151篇 |
2003年 | 167篇 |
2002年 | 147篇 |
2001年 | 109篇 |
2000年 | 103篇 |
1999年 | 54篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
1965年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有7908条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Ana Estevez Paula Jauregui Hibai Lopez‐Gonzalez 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2019,60(4):348-360
Gambling, video gaming, and Internet use are typically included as everyday activities that could potentially become behavioral addictions. There is growing evidence that views non‐substance related addictions as coping mechanisms, and considers that attachment styles are likely to play a pivotal role on the development of such addictions, especially on young people. Therefore, the present study aimed to: (1) explore the association between attachment, coping, and behavioral addictions (i.e., gambling disorder, video game addiction, and problematic Internet use); and (2) to analyze the mediating/moderating effect that coping has in the relationship of attachment and these behavioral addictions. A sample consisting of 472 students from secondary education (Mean age = 15.6; SD = 1.33; 51.6% females) was recruited. The findings showed a negative association between attachment and behavioral addictions. Most coping strategies were found to be associated with attachment styles, except for self‐critique and emotional avoidance. In particular, problem avoidance was significantly correlated to all behavioral addictions. In addition, self‐blame and problem solving were significantly correlated to video game addiction and problematic Internet use. Finally, coping was found to act as a mediator and moderator between attachment and video game addiction and problematic Internet use. 相似文献
102.
Diego Gomez‐Baya Marta Reis Margarida Gaspar de Matos 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2019,60(6):559-568
The framework of Positive Youth Development (PYD) arose from a strength‐based conception of the transition to adulthood. Although previous literature has provided evidence for some PYD correlates, little is known about gender differences in PYD and its outcomes in thriving and social engagement, which could improve interventions” effectiveness. Thus, this study aimed to examine gender differences in PYD dimensions (i.e., competence, confidence, connection, caring, and character), individual thriving (i.e., psychological adjustment, academic adjustment, and healthy lifestyles) and social engagement, and to examine the associations between them. Data from the ‘PYD in a Cross‐National Perspective Project” in Spain were collected by administering a paper‐based self‐report to students from high school and university. Results showed greater scores in connection, caring, and character in women, as well as greater social engagement and academic adjustment. Men presented higher competence and confidence, more frequent physical activity and better psychological adjustment. In addition, more PYD was related to greater social engagement, better psychological and academic adjustment, and healthier lifestyles. Some practical implications for program design may be derived, which underscore the need to promote the five PYD dimensions equally in female and male youths in order to improve their individual thriving and social engagement. 相似文献
103.
104.
105.
106.
107.
108.
Fernando Broncano‐Berrocal 《Philosophical Issues》2019,29(1):37-52
The lottery problem is the problem of explaining why mere reflection on the long odds that one will lose the lottery does not yield knowledge that one will lose. More generally, it is the problem of explaining why true beliefs merely formed on the basis of statistical evidence do not amount to knowledge. Some have thought that the lottery problem can be solved by appeal to a violation of the safety principle for knowledge, i.e., the principle that if S knows that p, not easily would S have believed that p without p being the case. Against the standard safety‐based solution, I argue that understanding safe belief as belief that directly covaries with the truth of what is believed in a suitably defined set of possible worlds forces safety theorists to make a series of theoretical choices that ultimately prevent a satisfactory solution to the problem. In this way, I analyze several safety principles that result from such choices—the paper thus gives valuable insights into the nature of safety—and explain why none solves the lottery problem, including their inability to explain away Gettierized lottery cases. On a more positive note, I show that there is a viable solution in terms of safety if we get rid of the unquestioned assumption that safe beliefs directly track the truth. The alternative is a conception of safe belief according to which what safe beliefs directly track is the appropriateness of the circumstances and, indirectly, the truth. The resulting safety principle, I argue, explains why mere statistical evidence is not a safe source of knowledge. 相似文献
109.
110.
Mario Sainz Rocío Martínez Miguel Moya Rosa Rodríguez‐Bailn 《International journal of psychology》2019,54(4):423-430
Differences between groups in socio‐economic status (SES) are becoming more salient nowadays. In this context, we examined the animalistic and mechanistic dehumanization that both low and high‐SES groups may experience respectively by conducting three studies. In study 1, we manipulated the SES of two fictitious groups (low vs. high‐SES) and measured the humanity ascribed to them. Results showed that the low‐SES group was animalized in comparison with the high‐SES group, which was mechanized. In study 2, we manipulated the humanity of two fictitious groups by describing them as animals or machines and measured the perceived SES of the groups. Participants tended to attribute lower SES to the group described as animals and higher SES to the group described as machines. Finally, in study 3, we used an Implicit Association Test to replicate the results of studies 1 and 2. Taken together, these studies show that low‐SES groups are considered as animal‐like whereas high‐SES groups are seen as robot‐like. We discuss the implications of these findings in relation to the justification of income inequality within our society. 相似文献