全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1596篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
1608篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 19篇 |
2023年 | 20篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 28篇 |
2020年 | 49篇 |
2019年 | 50篇 |
2018年 | 70篇 |
2017年 | 61篇 |
2016年 | 61篇 |
2015年 | 52篇 |
2014年 | 51篇 |
2013年 | 188篇 |
2012年 | 113篇 |
2011年 | 95篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 70篇 |
2007年 | 72篇 |
2006年 | 72篇 |
2005年 | 56篇 |
2004年 | 52篇 |
2003年 | 44篇 |
2002年 | 59篇 |
2001年 | 22篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 16篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1608条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
841.
This study was designed to investigate the relationship between midlife women's physical activity motives and their participation
in physical activity. Cross-sectional qualitative and quantitative data were collected from 59 midlife women, most of whom
were well-educated European-Americans (mean age = 45.6 years). Body-shape physical activity motives (i.e., motives related
to toning, shaping, and weight loss) were compared with all other types of physical activity motives combined. Participants
with body-shape motives were significantly less physically active than those with non-body-shape motives (p<.01). Negative affect toward physical activity was negatively associated with participation, but did not mediate the effect
of physical activity motives on participation. Body Mass Index (BMI) was not related to physical activity motives or participation.
Results suggest that body-shape motives might be associated with less physical activity participation than non-body-shape
motives among midlife women. 相似文献
842.
Michelle L. Thomas 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2006,28(2):201-210
The clients’ and therapists’ perceptions about the degree to which each of four common factors in therapy—extra-therapeutic factors; model/techniques; therapeutic alliance; and hope/expectancy—contribute to change in the therapeutic process were investigated in this study. In addition, the perceptions about the percentage of change attributed to clients and therapists were also explored. Results revealed that the therapists and clients have different perceptions on what factors contribute the most to change and that clients and therapists believe that the client contributes the most to change in a therapeutic process.M. L. Thomas, MSW, is a doctoral student in Marriage and Family Therapy at Florida State University. 相似文献
843.
Michelle Gonzales 《Dialog》2002,41(1):63-72
Latino/a Theology is rising among theologians of Latin American descent in the United States. This article describes five characteristics: 1) it is specific to the cultural context of marginalized Hispanic history; 2) it focuses on the theme of mestizaje/mulatez, the biological and cultural mixing of European Spanish with Africans and indigenous Americans; 3) it derives from popular religion; 4) it deals with everyday life, lo cotidiano; 5) like liberation theology, it emphasizes praxis. 相似文献
844.
845.
846.
Several long-term correlates of childhood sexual abuse (CSA) have been identified, including depression, fear, anxiety, guilt, anger, and poor interpersonal functioning (e.g., Browne and Finkelhor, 1986). However, little is know about how CSA survivors fare in old age. This article addresses the impact of unresolved CSA on late-life (emotional, cognitive, and behavioral) functioning. Three case studies of older adults ages 60 and older who were struggling with unresolved CSA are presented. Two observed stressors (loss of social support and declining health) and one developmental task (reminiscence or life-review) appear to be linked to exacerbated vulnerabilities in older survivors. Specifically, because CSA may arrest specific childhood developmental tasks (e.g., ability to form secure attachments, autonomy) or facilitate development of coping strategies that are maladaptive when outside of an abusive environment (e.g., generalized interpersonal distrust, avoidance, dissociation), the survivor may be at risk for developmental vulnerabilities for dysfunction in adulthood. These vulnerabilities may exacerbate unresolved CSA, particularly when the adult encounters stressors or developmental tasks that exceed his or her capacity for adaptive coping. Recommendations for mental health professionals and future research are suggested. 相似文献
847.
Adults (151 female, 130 male; 17.4% African American/Black, 48% Caucasian, 22.8% Latino/Hispanic, 11.7% “other”) assigned postdivorce parental care and custody for four combinations of traditional/nontraditional mothers and fathers described in vignettes of divorcing parents. Parental gender characteristics influenced the assignment of parental care and child custody to divorcing mothers and fathers described in the scenarios and interacted with child gender. Across scenarios, female participants assigned more parental care and custody to mothers than did male participants. When feminine qualities were paired with masculine qualities, greater custody was assigned to the parent described with feminine characteristics (whether a father or mother) than when that parent was described with masculine characteristics. The role of feminine gender characteristics for child custody and care was discussed with regard to maternal primacy and possible changes for father involvement in the aftermath of divorce. 相似文献
848.
John L. Bradshaw Michelle J. Howard Jane M. Pierson Jim Phillips Judy A. Bradshaw 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1992,44(3):509-528
The orienting of attention in space has not been considered in the tactile domain. This issue is examined using a modified version of a visual paradigm initially adopted by Posner, Snyder, and Davidson (1980), which manipulates the probability of a stimulus occurring at different spatial locations. Slower RTs at an unexpected stimulus location are thought to reflect the time required to shift attention from the expected to the unexpected location. In two experiments involving vibrotactile choice RT between left and right hands, the two hands were either crossed or uncrossed, and the hands were held both on the left side of the body, both on the right, or one on either side of the midline. There was no evidence to suggest that spatial location (left or right) affected the orienting of attention in the tactual modality. As predicted, RTs were slower when the arms were crossed compared with uncrossed, though this effect was smaller for the expected trials. A coding conflict hypothesis may explain both these findings, but the smaller effect in the expected trials may also reflect attentional factors. Both the relative and absolute location of the hands affected the magnitude of the crossed-arm effect and indicated that attention may play a role in the perceptual division of space into left and right sides. Possible reasons for hand or hemispace asymmetries in different simple and choice RT paradigms were discussed. 相似文献
849.
Gordon L. Flett Paul L. Hewitt Kirk R. Blankstein Melanie Solnik Michelle Van Brunschot 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》1996,14(4):245-274
The present research examined the relation between dimensions of perfectionism and self-appraised problem-solving behaviors and attitudes. Specifically, in two separate studies, we tested the hypothesis that socially prescribed perfectionism (i.e., the perception that others demand perfection from the self) is associated with poorer social problem-solving ability. In addition, measures of psychological adjustment were included in Study 2 so that we could (1) examine whether socially prescribed perfectionism and poorer problem-solving ability were still associated after removing variance associated with psychological distress; and (2) compare depression and anxiety in terms of their respective associations with social problem-solving ability. Correlational analyses of the data from both studies confirmed that socially prescribed perfectionism is associated with more negative self-perceptions of problem-solving orientation, and that the link between socially prescribed perfectionism and negative perceptions of problem-solving orientation remains present after removing variance due to levels of negative affectivity. Both depression and anxiety were associated with a negative problem-solving orientation, but only depression was associated with more negative appraisals of actual problem-solving skills. The results suggest that perceived exposure to imposed standards of perfection undermines the problem-solving process and that individuals with high levels of socially prescribed perfectionism are particularly in need of counseling interventions designed to provide a more positive problem-solving orientation.This research was supported by grants #410-91-0856 and #410-93-1256 from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1994 annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association in Providence, Rhode Island. The authors wish to thank Shawn Mosher for his assistance with the data collection. 相似文献