全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3961篇 |
免费 | 49篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
4011篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 36篇 |
2023年 | 40篇 |
2022年 | 38篇 |
2021年 | 66篇 |
2020年 | 88篇 |
2019年 | 104篇 |
2018年 | 142篇 |
2017年 | 138篇 |
2016年 | 155篇 |
2015年 | 109篇 |
2014年 | 121篇 |
2013年 | 480篇 |
2012年 | 238篇 |
2011年 | 204篇 |
2010年 | 151篇 |
2009年 | 137篇 |
2008年 | 176篇 |
2007年 | 173篇 |
2006年 | 164篇 |
2005年 | 137篇 |
2004年 | 127篇 |
2003年 | 123篇 |
2002年 | 124篇 |
2001年 | 64篇 |
2000年 | 56篇 |
1999年 | 53篇 |
1998年 | 38篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 45篇 |
1995年 | 34篇 |
1994年 | 30篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 41篇 |
1991年 | 25篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 26篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 26篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1969年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有4011条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
801.
Jean Blouin Bruce Bridgeman Normand Teasdale Chantal Bard Michelle Fleury 《Psychological research》1995,58(3):169-176
This experiment tested whether the perceived stability of the environment is altered when there is a combination of eye and visually open-loop hand movements toward a target displaced during the eye movements, i.e., during saccadic suppression. Visual-target eccentricity randomly decreased or increased during eye movements and subjects reported whether they perceived a target displacement or not, and if so, the direction of the displacement. Three experimental conditions, involving different combinations of eye and arm movements, were tested: (a) eye movements only; (b) simultaneous eye and rapid arm movements toward the target; and (c) simultaneous eye and arm movements with a restraint blocking the arm as soon as the hand left the starting position. The perceptual threshold of target displacements resulting in an increased target eccentricity was greater when subjects combined eye and arm movements toward the target object, specially for the no-restraint condition. Subjects corrected most of their arm trajectory toward the displaced target despite the short movement times (average MT = 189 ms). After the movements, the null error feedback of the hand's final position presumably overlapped the retino-oculomotor signal error and could be responsible for the deficient perception of target displacements. Thus, subjects interpreted the terminal hand positions as being within the range of the endpoint variability associated with the production of rapid arm movements rather than as a change of the environment. These results suggest that a natural strategy adopted for processing spatial information, especially in a competing situation, could favour a constancy tendency, avoiding systematic perception of a change of environment for any noise or variability at the central or peripheral levels. 相似文献
802.
Kernberg Otto Cohen Daniel Chiarandini Irene Cairo Winarick Kenneth Price Michelle Stern Donnel B. Rubin Jeffrey Ingram Douglas H. 《American journal of psychoanalysis》1997,57(4):381-387
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis - 相似文献
803.
EYEBLINK CLASSICAL CONDITIONING AND AWARENESS REVISITED 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Abstract— Dual-task performance was assessed in 140 adults during eyeblink classical conditioning (EBCC) and one of several secondary tasks (timed-interval tapping, recognition memory choice reaction time, video viewing). Four groups received paired-EBCC stimulus presentation and three groups received explicitly unpaired EBCC stimuli. Although the subjects were not laid about the conditioning task, they acquired conditioned responses (CRs) as normal levels. Postsession interviews probed participants' awareness of EBCC stimulus contingencies and production of CRs. Reported awareness of paired-EBCC stimulus contingencies and CK production was not related to actual EBCC performance Twenty-seven percent of the participants receiving explicitly unpaired stimuli reported a stimulus contingency when none existed. The dissociation between awareness and performances provides additional support for the categorization of simple EBCC as a form of nondeclarative learning. 相似文献
804.
A formal analysis is made of how to project an attribute criterion into the hierarchical classes model for object by attribute data proposed by De Boeck and Rosenberg. The projection is conceptualized as the prediction of the attribute criterion by means of a logical rule defined on the basis of attribute combinations from the model. Eliminative and constructive strategies are proposed to find logical rules with maximal predictive power and minimal formula complexity. Logical analyses of a real data set are reported and compared with a logistic regression to demonstrate the usefulness of the logical strategies, and to show the complementarity of logical and probabilistic approaches.The first suthor is Senior Research Assistant of the National Fund for Scientific Research (Belgium). We would like to thank the Editor, the reviewers, Seymour Rosenberg, and Luc Delbeke for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this article. 相似文献
805.
This article presents an overview of the development, implementation, and struggle to maintain guidance and counseling services at various levels within the Federal Republic of Germany. 相似文献
806.
The law of effect and avoidance: a quantitative relationship between response rate and shock-frequency reduction 总被引:8,自引:8,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

De Villiers PA 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1974,21(2):223-235
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the quantitative relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency in single and multiple variable-interval avoidance schedules. Responses cancelled delivery of shocks that were scheduled by variable-interval schedules. When shock-frequency reduction was taken as the measure of reinforcement, the relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency on single variable-interval avoidance schedules was accurately described by Herrnstein's (1970) equation for responding on single variable-interval schedules of positive reinforcement. On multiple variable-interval avoidance schedules with brief components, asymptotic relative response rate matched relative shock-frequency reduction. The results suggest that many interactions between response rates and shock-frequency reduction in avoidance can be understood within the framework of the generalized matching relation, as applied by Herrnstein (1970) to positive reinforcement. 相似文献
807.
808.
Samples of 56 male certified public accounting firm employees and 24 male accounting undergraduate students were given the CPI to test for subgroup differences in personality traits. The samples were also compared with eight occupational groups reported in the CPI manual. A statistical analysis of CPI scale mean score differences indicates a dichotomy of the accounting sample with the older accountants more conservative, conforming and restrictive than the younger accountants. 相似文献
809.
The first purpose of this bookbag and pokerchip experiment was to examine the influence of the sample size and the relative frequency of the red chips in the sample on the amount of conservatism. The second purpose was to replicate the results of DE SWART (1972), and to test his hypothesis that the slope B of the function: log Φ(T)=A+B(r−b), is linearly related to log [pr/(1−pr)], where pr represents the bag composition. 相似文献
810.
Reinforcement and response rate interaction in multiple random-interval avoidance schedules 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

De Villiers PA 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1972,18(3):499-507
Two experiments were conducted to investigate the interaction between response rate and reinforcement frequency in multiple random-interval avoidance schedules. Responses cancelled delivery of shocks that could be scheduled at different random intervals in each component. When shock-frequency reduction was taken as the measure of reinforcement, the relationship between response rate and frequency of reinforcement was described by the same equations used by Herrnstein (1970) to describe responding with positive reinforcement. 相似文献