全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12521篇 |
免费 | 178篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
12700篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 68篇 |
2022年 | 67篇 |
2021年 | 125篇 |
2020年 | 224篇 |
2019年 | 259篇 |
2018年 | 332篇 |
2017年 | 310篇 |
2016年 | 380篇 |
2015年 | 253篇 |
2014年 | 339篇 |
2013年 | 1318篇 |
2012年 | 568篇 |
2011年 | 560篇 |
2010年 | 418篇 |
2009年 | 419篇 |
2008年 | 585篇 |
2007年 | 567篇 |
2006年 | 504篇 |
2005年 | 431篇 |
2004年 | 466篇 |
2003年 | 423篇 |
2002年 | 413篇 |
2001年 | 179篇 |
2000年 | 199篇 |
1999年 | 186篇 |
1998年 | 194篇 |
1997年 | 159篇 |
1996年 | 109篇 |
1995年 | 148篇 |
1994年 | 144篇 |
1993年 | 133篇 |
1992年 | 124篇 |
1991年 | 112篇 |
1990年 | 109篇 |
1989年 | 106篇 |
1988年 | 110篇 |
1987年 | 107篇 |
1986年 | 85篇 |
1985年 | 103篇 |
1984年 | 116篇 |
1983年 | 98篇 |
1982年 | 110篇 |
1981年 | 118篇 |
1980年 | 106篇 |
1979年 | 102篇 |
1978年 | 111篇 |
1977年 | 89篇 |
1976年 | 93篇 |
1975年 | 64篇 |
1974年 | 76篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
951.
Studies of electrophysiological indices of performance monitoring, such as the error-related negativity (ERN), posterror positivity
(Pe), and N2 components of the event-related potential (ERP), suggest that increased ERN and Pe amplitudes and decreased N2
amplitudes are associated with better cognitive flexibility and cognitive control abilities; however, few studies have directly
examined the relationship between cognitive performance and ERP indices of performance monitoring. We examined the neuropsychological
profile of 89 healthy individuals who performed a modified flanker task. The neuropsychological domains tested included memory,
verbal fluency, and attention/executive functioning. Pearson’s correlations and multiple regression analyses showed a significant
relationship between measures of attention/executive functioning and ERN amplitude, even when negative affect, reaction time
interference, and posterror slowing were controlled. N2 amplitude related only to posterror slowing. The amplitude of the
Pe was not significantly related to any cognitive domains. These findings are consistent with recent work indicating that
performance monitoring requires attention skills and cognitive flexibility. Implications for the conflict-monitoring and reinforcement-learning
theories are discussed. 相似文献
952.
Jolanda Jetten Michael T. Schmitt Blake M. McKimmie 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2005,41(2):208-215
We examined (N = 76) how social creativity strategies such as intergroup differentiation and intragroup respect suppress the negative impact of threat to an ingroup’s value on group identification. Threat was manipulated through false feedback concerning how other groups perceived an ingroup. Both intergroup differentiation and intragroup respect were higher when participants learned that the ingroup was devalued compared to when it was valued. Mediational analyses demonstrated that these factors suppressed the direct negative relationship between value threat and group identification. Discussion focused on the consequences of these social creativity strategies for group identification and collective action. 相似文献
953.
954.
955.
956.
957.
The Democratic Republic of Congo has vast natural resources, many of which are regularly exploited by the electronics industry. Unfortunately, in addition to these resources, there are widespread human rights abuses committed by armed groups entrenched in the eastern part of the Democratic Republic of Congo. These armed groups are using profits from these minerals as a source of funding. Their human rights abuses have led to a growing humanitarian interest in the region and prompted the international community to action. This paper explores the conflicts in the Democratic Republic of Congo, provides an understanding of the link between human rights abuses and conflict minerals, and interprets and critiques the legal actions of the international community. 相似文献
958.
It is well known that minimum-diameter partitioning of symmetric dissimilarity matrices can be framed within the context of coloring the vertices of a graph. Although confusion data are typically represented in the form of asymmetric similarity matrices, they are also amenable to a graph-coloring perspective. In this paper, we propose the integration of the minimum-diameter partitioning method with a neighborhood-based coloring approach for analyzing digraphs corresponding to confusion data. This procedure is capable of producing minimum-diameter partitions with the added desirable property that vertices with the same color have similar in-neighborhoods (i.e., directed edges entering the vertex) and out-neighborhoods (i.e., directed edges exiting the vertex) for the digraph corresponding to the minimum partition diameter. 相似文献
959.
960.
In this study, we examined the relationship of the MCMI-III (Millon, Davis, & Millon, 1997; Millon, Millon, & Davis, 1994) modifier indices and personality disorder scales to the validity and basic clinical scales of the MMPI-2 (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989). The MCMI-III modifier indices highly correlated with all of the MMPI-2 validity scales except for the F(p) scale. Similarly, the MCMI-III personality disorder scales strongly covaried with the MMPI-2 validity and clinical scales except for the F(p) and 5 (Mf) scales. A factor analysis with Promax rotation revealed substantial relationships between the MMPI-2 and MCMI-III. However, the MMPI-2 F(p) scale did not tend to correlate with MMPI-2 or MCMI-III scales, indicating that F(p) scale variance was largely independent of other scales. The results suggest that clinicians should consider the interrelationship between personality characteristics and dissimulation. 相似文献