全文获取类型
收费全文 | 13186篇 |
免费 | 197篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
13384篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 72篇 |
2022年 | 70篇 |
2021年 | 125篇 |
2020年 | 231篇 |
2019年 | 277篇 |
2018年 | 346篇 |
2017年 | 328篇 |
2016年 | 404篇 |
2015年 | 271篇 |
2014年 | 366篇 |
2013年 | 1371篇 |
2012年 | 585篇 |
2011年 | 580篇 |
2010年 | 432篇 |
2009年 | 441篇 |
2008年 | 612篇 |
2007年 | 578篇 |
2006年 | 518篇 |
2005年 | 445篇 |
2004年 | 482篇 |
2003年 | 437篇 |
2002年 | 426篇 |
2001年 | 197篇 |
2000年 | 218篇 |
1999年 | 206篇 |
1998年 | 203篇 |
1997年 | 174篇 |
1996年 | 120篇 |
1995年 | 155篇 |
1994年 | 149篇 |
1993年 | 143篇 |
1992年 | 138篇 |
1991年 | 128篇 |
1990年 | 115篇 |
1989年 | 115篇 |
1988年 | 116篇 |
1987年 | 112篇 |
1986年 | 93篇 |
1985年 | 119篇 |
1984年 | 128篇 |
1983年 | 111篇 |
1982年 | 115篇 |
1981年 | 124篇 |
1980年 | 110篇 |
1979年 | 104篇 |
1978年 | 124篇 |
1977年 | 96篇 |
1976年 | 97篇 |
1975年 | 67篇 |
1974年 | 89篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
291.
Younkin Felisha L. Firmin Michael W. Parrill Rachel L. Smith Alexis D. Krauss Zachary J. Jones Theresa M. 《Journal of religion and health》2021,60(3):2125-2137
Journal of Religion and Health - Our interdisciplinary team (which included professionals from nursing, pharmacy, allied health, and psychology) conducted in-depth, semi-structured interviews with... 相似文献
292.
293.
Michael J. Boulton 《Aggressive behavior》1996,22(4):271-287
Study 1 compared the extent to which 8- and 11-year-old girls and boys (N = 86) participated in specific types of rough-and-tumble play. It used an observational methodology. The data were used to test two prominent hypotheses about the evolutionary function of this general category of play behaviour that have been applied to children, especially boys, of this age. One is that rough-and-tumble play provides practice for the development of real fighting skills and the other that it serves as a safe way to establish/display social dominance. Both hypotheses predict that boys will engage in more rough-and-tumble play than girls, especially those types that are used in fighting/dominance contests. Boys were found to engage in significantly more chase initiation activities, more bouts of brief rough-and-tumble play, more bouts of restraining and more bouts of boxing/hitting than girls. These data provide some support for the two hypotheses, although significant sex differences were not found for all types of rough-and-tumble play observed. No significant age differences were obtained, suggesting that the two hypotheses may be applicable throughout the 8- to 11-year-old period and not just at the end of it as previous research had suggested. Study 2 presented observational data concerning the motor patterns used in aggressive fighting. In all but one case, there were no significant differences in the extent to which 8/9- and 10/11-year-old girls and boys employed wrestling, hitting and restraining, supporting the view that the practice fighting hypothesis is relevant throughout the 8-11-year-old period. It was argued that age and sex differences provide a useful means of scrutinising functional hypotheses, and that splitting behavioural categories that contain disparate action patterns facilitates more refined tests of those hypotheses. © 1996 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
294.
Michael M. Kostreva Marina V. Polyashuk 《Journal of Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis》1993,2(3):159-166
A mathematical formulation for a dilemma (i.e. a situation in which a very difficult choice between two alternatives must be made) is given. Multiple-objective programming is assumed as the model for a single decision maker. It is shown that, in principle, any dilemma is resolvable. Furthermore, there is always more than one way in which to resolve a dilemma. Illustrative examples from non-convex optimization are presented. 相似文献
295.
International Journal for Philosophy of Religion - 相似文献
296.
Michael Hannon 《Philosophy and phenomenological research》2020,101(3):591-611
Epistemic democracy is standardly characterized in terms of “aiming at truth”. This presupposes a veritistic conception of epistemic value, according to which truth is the fundamental epistemic goal. I will raise an objection to the standard (veritistic) account of epistemic democracy, focusing specifically on deliberative democracy. I then propose a version of deliberative democracy that is grounded in non-veritistic epistemic goals. In particular, I argue that deliberation is valuable because it facilitates empathetic understanding. I claim that empathetic understanding is an epistemic good that doesn't have truth as its primary goal. 相似文献
297.
Synthese - This paper seeks to answer the following question: What is a minimal set of entities that form an ontology of the natural world, given our well-established physical theories? The... 相似文献
298.
Michael Potegal 《Aggressive behavior》1979,5(4):353-373
Field observations of “surplus killing” and laboratory studies of operant performance rewarded by prey-killing opportunities suggest that predatory behavior is positively reinforcing. Similarly, both repeated encounter and operant performance studies suggest that intraspecific aggression can be positively reinforcing for successful aggressors. While a few studies suggest that defensive aggression under aversive conditions may also be positively reinforcing, it appears that when appropriate response modes are available escape and/or avoidance are preferred to attack. Studies of the reinforcing properties of aggression-eliciting brain stimulation are in general agreement with these conclusions, but methodological problems with these latter observations render them less compelling. The progressive escalation of aggression seen in “warm-up effects” of birds and fish, “priming effects” of mice, and ecstatic violence of humans may be analogous processes based on the positively self-reinforcing characteristics of some kinds of aggression. The transient reductions of aggression which appear as refractory periods and satiation effects in a variety of species may reflect temporary reductions in the reinforcing value of aggression. All these temporal effects must be considered in the evaluation of experiments on the reinforcing value of aggression. More generally, it is possible that these temporal fluctuations reflect the operation of common motivational processes (aggressive states) which regulate overt aggression by changing its reinforcing value. 相似文献
299.
300.
Defensive aggression was examined in seven species of terns during the hatching period when young are most vulnerable and adults most responsive. Aggression toward an unfamiliar person varied with respect to species, flush distance, approach distance, number of birds overhead and diving, and number of dives per 2 min for all birds and the nest owner. Defensive aggression involved birds circling over and diving at the experimenter. Generally, flush distance was negatively correlated with nest density, while the number of birds diving was positively correlated with the nest density. The number of birds diving was positively related to the number of dives, dives by the owner, and the number overhead. 相似文献