首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   21474篇
  免费   1031篇
  国内免费   10篇
  2023年   111篇
  2021年   201篇
  2020年   371篇
  2019年   445篇
  2018年   587篇
  2017年   597篇
  2016年   665篇
  2015年   466篇
  2014年   598篇
  2013年   2392篇
  2012年   955篇
  2011年   995篇
  2010年   687篇
  2009年   702篇
  2008年   947篇
  2007年   936篇
  2006年   820篇
  2005年   753篇
  2004年   761篇
  2003年   705篇
  2002年   718篇
  2001年   370篇
  2000年   386篇
  1999年   354篇
  1998年   320篇
  1997年   268篇
  1996年   233篇
  1995年   269篇
  1994年   257篇
  1993年   223篇
  1992年   246篇
  1991年   216篇
  1990年   207篇
  1989年   197篇
  1988年   185篇
  1987年   181篇
  1986年   173篇
  1985年   203篇
  1984年   214篇
  1983年   177篇
  1982年   213篇
  1981年   216篇
  1980年   179篇
  1979年   189篇
  1978年   178篇
  1977年   179篇
  1976年   159篇
  1975年   142篇
  1974年   147篇
  1973年   129篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
23.
24.
25.
Recent studies demonstrating that multiple meanings of an ambiguous word are initially accessed even when only one reading is syntactically appropriate with the preceding context can be criticized on at least two grounds. First, many of the syntactic contexts used were not truly restrictive, and, secondly, subjects may not have had time to integrate the context before processing the ambiguous word. In the present study, subjects listened to a sentence ending in an ambiguous word and then made a lexical decision to a target related to either the appropriate or inappropriate reading. Contexts were completely restrictive, and a pause was introduced between the context and the ambiguous word. Multiple access still obtained, providing further support for the claim that lexical access is not guided by syntactic context.  相似文献   
26.
Informed by theory and research on attributions and narrative persuasion, we compared the effectiveness of narrative and nonnarrative messages in changing attributions of responsibility for causes and solutions related to obesity in the United States. We randomly assigned 500 adults to view one of three messages (narrative, evidence, and a hybrid of the two) emphasizing environmental causes of obesity, or a no‐exposure control condition. The narrative condition increased the belief that societal actors (government, employers) are responsible for addressing obesity, but only among liberals. This pattern of results was partially explained by the success of the narrative condition in reducing reactive counterarguing, relative to the evidence condition, among liberals. We conclude with theoretical and practical implications.  相似文献   
27.
28.
29.
30.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号