全文获取类型
收费全文 | 14022篇 |
免费 | 198篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
14221篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 77篇 |
2022年 | 80篇 |
2021年 | 141篇 |
2020年 | 256篇 |
2019年 | 307篇 |
2018年 | 396篇 |
2017年 | 375篇 |
2016年 | 443篇 |
2015年 | 288篇 |
2014年 | 396篇 |
2013年 | 1462篇 |
2012年 | 650篇 |
2011年 | 636篇 |
2010年 | 457篇 |
2009年 | 466篇 |
2008年 | 649篇 |
2007年 | 636篇 |
2006年 | 548篇 |
2005年 | 471篇 |
2004年 | 509篇 |
2003年 | 475篇 |
2002年 | 470篇 |
2001年 | 210篇 |
2000年 | 222篇 |
1999年 | 204篇 |
1998年 | 212篇 |
1997年 | 172篇 |
1996年 | 126篇 |
1995年 | 163篇 |
1994年 | 161篇 |
1993年 | 141篇 |
1992年 | 139篇 |
1991年 | 123篇 |
1990年 | 122篇 |
1989年 | 118篇 |
1988年 | 124篇 |
1987年 | 117篇 |
1986年 | 94篇 |
1985年 | 111篇 |
1984年 | 125篇 |
1983年 | 103篇 |
1982年 | 118篇 |
1981年 | 124篇 |
1980年 | 114篇 |
1979年 | 108篇 |
1978年 | 123篇 |
1977年 | 93篇 |
1976年 | 100篇 |
1975年 | 66篇 |
1974年 | 79篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Michael Slote 《Dao》2018,17(1):1-11
The East Asian notion of a heart-mind (Chinese xin 心, Japanese kokoro こころ, and Korean maum ??) is arguably more accurate to our psychology than the Western term “mind” and its equivalents are: the latter term implies the possibility of psychological functioning in the absence of all emotion, and it can be shown that that is impossible. But then it turns out that we can update the traditional Chinese notions of yin 陰 and yang 陽 in such a way as to help us philosophically explain how our functioning psychology involves emotion and why any possible psychology has to rest on such a basis. Yin-yang is the essence of heart-mind, and heart-mind is essential to any functioning psychology. 相似文献
992.
Manuel Rausch Sebastian Hellmann Michael Zehetleitner 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2018,80(1):134-154
How do human observers determine their degree of belief that they are correct in a decision about a visual stimulus—that is, their confidence? According to prominent theories of confidence, the quality of stimulation should be positively related to confidence in correct decisions, and negatively to confidence in incorrect decisions. However, in a backward-masked orientation task with a varying stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA), we observed that confidence in incorrect decisions also increased with stimulus quality. Model fitting to our decision and confidence data revealed that the best explanation for the present data was the new weighted evidence-and-visibility model, according to which confidence is determined by evidence about the orientation as well as by the general visibility of the stimulus. Signal detection models, postdecisional accumulation models, two-channel models, and decision-time-based models were all unable to explain the pattern of confidence as a function of SOA and decision correctness. We suggest that the metacognitive system combines several cues related to the correctness of a decision about a visual stimulus in order to calculate decision confidence. 相似文献
993.
James M. Broadway Michael J. Frank James F. Cavanagh 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2018,18(3):509-520
The interplay of dopaminergic striatal D1–D2 circuits is thought to support working memory (WM) by selectively filtering information that is to be remembered versus information to be ignored. To test this theory, we conducted an experiment in which healthy participants performed a visuospatial working memory (VSWM) task after ingesting the D2-receptor agonist cabergoline (or placebo), in a randomized, double-blinded, crossover design. Results showed greater interference from distractors under cabergoline, particularly for individuals with higher baseline dopamine (indicated by WM span). These findings support computational theories of striatal D1–D2 function during WM encoding and distractor-filtering, and provide new evidence for interactive cortico-striatal systems that support VSWM capacity and their dependence on WM span. 相似文献
994.
This study examined whether differences in the amount of information provided to men and women, in the form of verbal instruction, influenced their encoding during a reorientation task. When a navigator needs to orient, featural (e.g., colour or texture) and geometry (e.g., metric information) are used to determine which direction to begin traveling. The current study used a spatial reorientation task to examine how men and women use featural and geometric cues and whether the content of the task’s instructions influenced how these cues were used. Participants were trained to find a target location in a rectangular room with distinctive objects situated at each corner. Once the participants were accurately locating the target, various tests manipulating the spatial information were conducted. We found both men and women encoded the featural cues, and even though the features provided reliable information, participants generally showed an encoding of geometry. However, when participants were not provided with any information about the spatial aspects of the task in the instructions, they failed to encode geometry. We also found that women used distant featural cues as landmarks when the featural cue closest to the target was removed, whereas men did not. Yet, when the two types of cues were placed in conflict, both sexes weighed featural cues more heavily than geometric cues. The content of the task instructions also influenced how cues were relied upon in this conflict situation. Our results have important implications for our understanding of how spatial cues are used for reorientation. 相似文献
995.
The development of cognitive models involves the creative scientific formalization of assumptions, based on theory, observation, and other relevant information. In the Bayesian approach to implementing, testing, and using cognitive models, assumptions can influence both the likelihood function of the model, usually corresponding to assumptions about psychological processes, and the prior distribution over model parameters, usually corresponding to assumptions about the psychological variables that influence those processes. The specification of the prior is unique to the Bayesian context, but often raises concerns that lead to the use of vague or non-informative priors in cognitive modeling. Sometimes the concerns stem from philosophical objections, but more often practical difficulties with how priors should be determined are the stumbling block. We survey several sources of information that can help to specify priors for cognitive models, discuss some of the methods by which this information can be formalized in a prior distribution, and identify a number of benefits of including informative priors in cognitive modeling. Our discussion is based on three illustrative cognitive models, involving memory retention, categorization, and decision making. 相似文献
996.
Meghan Hegarty-Craver Kristin H. Gilchrist Cathi B. Propper Gregory F. Lewis Samuel J. DeFilipp Jennifer L. Coffman Michael T. Willoughby 《Behavior research methods》2018,50(5):1816-1823
Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) is a quantitative metric that reflects autonomic nervous system regulation and provides a physiological marker of attentional engagement that supports cognitive and affective regulatory processes. RSA can be added to executive function (EF) assessments with minimal participant burden because of the commercial availability of lightweight, wearable electrocardiogram (ECG) sensors. However, the inclusion of RSA data in large data collection efforts has been hindered by the time-intensive processing of RSA. In this study we evaluated the performance of an automated RSA-scoring method in the context of an EF study in preschool-aged children. The absolute differences in RSA across both scoring methods were small (mean RSA differences = –0.02–0.10), with little to no evidence of bias for the automated relative to the hand-scoring approach. Moreover, the relative rank-ordering of RSA across both scoring methods was strong (rs = .96–.99). Reliable changes in RSA from baseline to the EF task were highly similar across both scoring methods (96%–100% absolute agreement; Kappa = .83–1.0). On the basis of these findings, the automated RSA algorithm appears to be a suitable substitute for hand-scoring in the context of EF assessment. 相似文献
997.
Marieke M. J. W. van Rooij Luis H. Favela MaryLauren Malone Michael J. Richardson 《Ecological Psychology》2013,25(3):293-303
Individuals make decisions under uncertainty every day. Decisions are based on incomplete information concerning the potential outcome or the predicted likelihood with which events occur. In addition, individuals' choices often deviate from the rational or mathematically objective solution. Accordingly, the dynamics of human decision making are difficult to capture using conventional, linear mathematical models. Here, we present data from a 2-choice task with variable risk between sure loss and risky loss to illustrate how a simple nonlinear dynamical system can be employed to capture the dynamics of human decision making under uncertainty (i.e., multistability, bifurcations). We test the feasibility of this model quantitatively and demonstrate how the model can account for up to 86% of the observed choice behavior. The implications of using dynamical models for explaining the nonlinear complexities of human decision making are discussed as well as the degree to which the theory of nonlinear dynamical systems might offer an alternative framework for understanding human decision making processes. 相似文献
998.
Diane M. Ste-Marie Amanda Rymal Kelly Vertes Rose Martini 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2013,25(3):292-307
The observation of oneself on video that has been edited to show a performance level higher than one can actually perform is a feedforward form of modeling, termed self-modeling (SM; Dowrick, 1999). In this research, gymnasts alternated between viewing and not viewing a SM video during their competitive season. Results showed that gymnasts attained significantly higher beam scores when they viewed the video versus when they did not. No differences in self-efficacy were observed using a quantitative measure; however, a qualitative analysis of interviews based on Zimmerman's (2000) model, indicated that a number of self-regulatory processes, including self-efficacy, were employed. 相似文献
999.
1000.