首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   194747篇
  免费   8552篇
  国内免费   162篇
  2020年   2885篇
  2019年   3560篇
  2018年   3881篇
  2017年   4284篇
  2016年   4906篇
  2015年   4026篇
  2014年   4922篇
  2013年   23939篇
  2012年   5322篇
  2011年   4668篇
  2010年   4298篇
  2009年   5056篇
  2008年   4810篇
  2007年   4434篇
  2006年   4799篇
  2005年   4642篇
  2004年   4151篇
  2003年   3736篇
  2002年   3584篇
  2001年   4182篇
  2000年   4035篇
  1999年   3747篇
  1998年   2956篇
  1997年   2780篇
  1996年   2678篇
  1995年   2503篇
  1994年   2471篇
  1993年   2434篇
  1992年   3093篇
  1991年   2906篇
  1990年   2739篇
  1989年   2602篇
  1988年   2585篇
  1987年   2584篇
  1986年   2599篇
  1985年   2806篇
  1984年   2812篇
  1983年   2538篇
  1982年   2485篇
  1981年   2456篇
  1980年   2284篇
  1979年   2587篇
  1978年   2381篇
  1977年   2270篇
  1976年   2092篇
  1975年   2279篇
  1974年   2361篇
  1973年   2291篇
  1972年   1872篇
  1971年   1766篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
211.
212.
213.
214.
215.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
216.
217.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
218.
An electronic apparatus using infrared beams for monitoring the movements of individual bees under dark conditions is described. The searching behavior of workers in an arena was monitored over 2-h periods. Mean ambulatory velocity for one bee over a distance of 100 mm was 45.6±1.51 mm/sec (n=45). Thigmokinesis and temporal activity patterns are illustrated.  相似文献   
219.
220.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号