全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12695篇 |
免费 | 179篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
12875篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 71篇 |
2022年 | 70篇 |
2021年 | 126篇 |
2020年 | 227篇 |
2019年 | 264篇 |
2018年 | 337篇 |
2017年 | 315篇 |
2016年 | 387篇 |
2015年 | 259篇 |
2014年 | 343篇 |
2013年 | 1336篇 |
2012年 | 578篇 |
2011年 | 571篇 |
2010年 | 425篇 |
2009年 | 420篇 |
2008年 | 587篇 |
2007年 | 576篇 |
2006年 | 508篇 |
2005年 | 433篇 |
2004年 | 472篇 |
2003年 | 427篇 |
2002年 | 416篇 |
2001年 | 182篇 |
2000年 | 200篇 |
1999年 | 189篇 |
1998年 | 193篇 |
1997年 | 161篇 |
1996年 | 111篇 |
1995年 | 149篇 |
1994年 | 145篇 |
1993年 | 133篇 |
1992年 | 126篇 |
1991年 | 115篇 |
1990年 | 110篇 |
1989年 | 108篇 |
1988年 | 113篇 |
1987年 | 109篇 |
1986年 | 87篇 |
1985年 | 103篇 |
1984年 | 119篇 |
1983年 | 99篇 |
1982年 | 110篇 |
1981年 | 118篇 |
1980年 | 106篇 |
1979年 | 103篇 |
1978年 | 112篇 |
1977年 | 89篇 |
1976年 | 93篇 |
1975年 | 67篇 |
1974年 | 79篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
951.
952.
Cultural differences in process-focused versus person-focused themes were examined using both cultural artifacts (Study 1) and self-reports (Study 2). In Study 1, the contents of Chinese and American graduation and encouragement cards were analyzed for their relative emphasis on person- versus process-focused themes. Person-focused themes center on recipients' stable traits and abilities, and their emotional well-being. In contrast, process-focused themes dwell on the recipients' hard work and effort, and emphasize the importance of continued self-improvement and growth. Messages on Chinese cards were significantly more process than person focused. The reverse was true of American cards. Chinese cards also contained more process-focused (e.g., winding roads) than person-focused images (e.g., student standing on a pedestal). American cards contained more person-focused than process-focused images, although this difference was significant only among encouragement cards. In Study 2, we presented Chinese and American participants with graduation card messages differing in focus. Chinese parents indicated that they would be more likely to select and Chinese students indicated that they would be more likely to receive process- than person-themed graduation card messages. American parents and students showed no effects of message focus. The findings illustrate how cultural beliefs are reflected in cultural artifacts and personal preferences. 相似文献
953.
Beran MJ 《Animal cognition》2011,14(5):637-645
The isolation effect (or von Restorff effect) occurs when one item in a to-be-remembered list is distinctive from all remaining
items, and memory for that item is enhanced. Four chimpanzees were presented with a serial list of four photographs. In the
homogeneous condition, all list items were from the same semantic category (e.g., four fruits). In the isolate condition,
three items were from the same category, but the fourth item (the isolate) was from a different category (e.g., three fruits
and one toy). Then, two photographs were presented, and the chimpanzees had to select the one that was from the list. Two
of four chimpanzees were significantly more likely to select a correct isolate item than an item from the same list position
in the homogeneous condition for at least some list positions. This facilitation in performance was for isolate items only,
as presenting an isolate item in a list did not facilitate greater recognition of other list items compared to the homogeneous
condition. These results indicated that some chimpanzees perceived the semantic categories of the photographs, and categorization
of photographs led to the isolation effect. Thus, chimpanzees may share with humans some aspects of memory organization that
involve spontaneously categorizing visual stimuli and recognizing categorically unique stimuli. 相似文献
954.
Marianne Steiner Mathias Allemand Michael E. McCullough 《Journal of research in personality》2011,(6):670-678
The present study examined age differences in the disposition to forgive others and the role of interpersonal transgression frequency and intensity. Data from a representative cross-sectional sample of Swiss adults (N = 451, age: 20–83 years) were used. Participants completed a self-report measure of forgivingness and indicated whether and how intense they have experienced different types of interpersonal transgressions during the past 12 months. Results indicate that older adults were, on average, more willing to forgive others than younger adults. Frequency and intensity of transgressions were negatively related with age. Moreover, the results show that transgression frequency and intensity explained, in part, age differences in forgivingness. Future directions concerning the meaning of age differences in forgivingness are discussed. 相似文献
955.
Three hundred and four participants in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 study took a validated IQ-type test at age 11 years and a battery of cognitive tests at age 70 years. Three tests of health literacy were completed at age 72 years; the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM), the Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults (S-TOFHLA), and the Newest Vital Sign (NVS). Participants who had a lower childhood IQ exhibited poorer performance on all three tests of health literacy taken in older adulthood. Relative cognitive change from age 11 to 70 and education were also important factors influencing performance on health literacy tasks, independent of childhood cognitive ability. It is important to understand the determinants of low health literacy in order to support individuals in managing their own health. 相似文献
956.
Birnbaum MH 《Psychological review》2011,118(4):675-83; discussion 684-8
This article contrasts 2 approaches to analyzing transitivity of preference and other behavioral properties in choice data. The approach of Regenwetter, Dana, and Davis-Stober (see record 2011-00732-003) assumes that on each choice, a decision maker samples randomly from a mixture of preference orders to determine whether A is preferred to B. In contrast, Birnbaum and Gutierrez (2007) assumed that within each block of trials, the decision maker has a true set of preferences and that random errors generate variability of response. In this latter approach, preferences are allowed to differ between people; within-person, they might differ between repetition blocks. Both approaches allow mixtures of preferences, both assume a type of independence, and both yield statistical tests. They differ with respect to the locus of independence in the data. The approaches also differ in the criterion for assessing the success of the models. Regenwetter et al. fitted only marginal choice proportions and assumed that choices are independent, which means that a mixture cannot be identified from the data. Birnbaum and Gutierrez fitted choice combinations with replications; their approach allows estimation of the probabilities in the mixture. It is suggested that researchers should separate tests of the stochastic model from the test of transitivity. Evidence testing independence and stationarity assumptions is presented. Available data appear to fit the assumption that errors are independent better than they fit the assumption that choices are independent. 相似文献
957.
Subašić E Schmitt MT Reynolds KJ 《The British journal of social psychology / the British Psychological Society》2011,50(4):707-725
Common experience of injustice can be a potent motivator of collective action and efforts to achieve social change - and of such efforts becoming more widespread. In this research, we propose that the effects of co-victimization on collective action are a function of inclusive social identity. Experiment 1 (N= 61) demonstrated that while presence (compared to absence) of co-victimization positively predicted consumer (i.e., participants) willingness to act collectively in solidarity with sweatshop workers, this effect was mediated by inclusive social identity. In Experiment 2 (N= 120), the salience of inclusive social identity was experimentally manipulated and interacted with co-victimization to predict collective action. When inclusive social identity was salient, co-victimization enhanced collective action, including willingness to pay extra for products made ethically and in support of fair wages for workers. In contrast, collective action was attenuated when co-victimization took place in the absence of inclusive social identity. Implications for understanding when co-victimization is transformed into common fate and political solidarity with the disadvantaged are discussed. 相似文献
958.
Schleyer M Saumweber T Nahrendorf W Fischer B von Alpen D Pauls D Thum A Gerber B 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2011,18(10):639-653
Drosophila larvae combine a numerically simple brain, a correspondingly moderate behavioral complexity, and the availability of a rich toolbox for transgenic manipulation. This makes them attractive as a study case when trying to achieve a circuit-level understanding of behavior organization. From a series of behavioral experiments, we suggest a circuitry of chemosensory processing, odor-tastant memory trace formation, and the "decision" process to behaviorally express these memory traces--or not. The model incorporates statements about the neuronal organization of innate vs. conditioned chemosensory behavior, and the types of interaction between olfactory and gustatory pathways during the establishment as well as the behavioral expression of odor-tastant memory traces. It in particular suggests that innate olfactory behavior is responsive in nature, whereas conditioned olfactory behavior is captured better when seen as an action in pursuit of its outcome. It incorporates the available neuroanatomical and behavioral data and thus should be useful as scaffold for the ongoing investigations of the chemo-behavioral system in larval Drosophila. 相似文献
959.
Michael J. Raven 《Philosophia》2011,39(4):751-757
Aristotelian relativism about the future (as recently defended by MacFarlane (2003)) claims that a prediction made on Monday, such as ‘It will rain’, can be indeterminate on Monday but determinate on Tuesday.
A serious objection to this intuitively appealing view is that it cannot coherently be attested: for if it is attested on
Monday, then our blindness to what the future holds precludes attesting that the prediction is determinate on Tuesday, and
if it is attested on Tuesday (when, suppose, it rains), then the fact that it rains precludes attesting that the prediction
is indeterminate on Monday. In this paper, I focus on Moruzzi and Wright (2009)’s recent development of this objection and argue that it fails. This result removes a major obstacle to defending the Aristotelian
view. 相似文献
960.
Jesse M. Crosby Scott C. Bates Michael P. Twohig 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2011,30(2):117-129
A relationship between perfectionism and religiosity has been suggested in the literature, and this relationship is clarified
further when the adaptive and maladaptive dimensions of both constructs are compared. Literature in both areas implicates
the idea of a rigid and inflexible personality style that may explain why well meaning high standards can be associated with
negative outcomes such as perfectionism. This investigation examined the relationship of perfectionism and religiosity, using
adaptive and maladaptive dimensions, as mediated by psychological inflexibility. Validated measures of perfectionism, religiosity,
and psychological inflexibility were given to 376 undergraduate college students in an anonymous online survey. Adaptive perfectionism
(high standards) was found to be significantly correlated (r = .26, p < .01, two-tailed) with adaptive religiosity (intrinsic orientation). Maladaptive perfectionism (discrepancy) was found to
be significantly correlated (r = .13, p < .05, two-tailed) with maladaptive religiosity (extrinsic orientation). Psychological inflexibility was found to be significantly
correlated with the maladaptive dimensions of both perfectionism and religiosity. It was also shown to mediate the relationship
between maladaptive (extrinsic) religiosity and maladaptive (discrepancy) perfectionism. Implications and future directions
are discussed. 相似文献