全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12512篇 |
免费 | 178篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
12691篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 68篇 |
2022年 | 67篇 |
2021年 | 125篇 |
2020年 | 224篇 |
2019年 | 258篇 |
2018年 | 332篇 |
2017年 | 310篇 |
2016年 | 380篇 |
2015年 | 253篇 |
2014年 | 339篇 |
2013年 | 1317篇 |
2012年 | 568篇 |
2011年 | 560篇 |
2010年 | 418篇 |
2009年 | 419篇 |
2008年 | 585篇 |
2007年 | 567篇 |
2006年 | 504篇 |
2005年 | 431篇 |
2004年 | 466篇 |
2003年 | 423篇 |
2002年 | 413篇 |
2001年 | 179篇 |
2000年 | 196篇 |
1999年 | 185篇 |
1998年 | 191篇 |
1997年 | 159篇 |
1996年 | 108篇 |
1995年 | 148篇 |
1994年 | 144篇 |
1993年 | 133篇 |
1992年 | 124篇 |
1991年 | 112篇 |
1990年 | 110篇 |
1989年 | 106篇 |
1988年 | 110篇 |
1987年 | 107篇 |
1986年 | 85篇 |
1985年 | 103篇 |
1984年 | 116篇 |
1983年 | 98篇 |
1982年 | 110篇 |
1981年 | 118篇 |
1980年 | 106篇 |
1979年 | 102篇 |
1978年 | 111篇 |
1977年 | 89篇 |
1976年 | 93篇 |
1975年 | 64篇 |
1974年 | 76篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
931.
Teresa M. Preddy Paula J. Fite Amber R. Wimsatt Michael L. Vitulano Alden E. Gaertner 《Journal of child and family studies》2014,23(3):510-520
Although the link between reactively aggressive behavior and depressive symptoms in childhood has been demonstrated in previous work, few studies have examined how peer factors may influence this association. Examining the role of peers in the link between reactive aggression and depressive symptoms is necessary, as peers have been found to buffer the impact of factors that contribute to depressive symptoms in childhood. Accordingly, we examined whether intimate exchange with a best friend moderated the association between reactive aggression and depression in childhood in a sample of 115 children (aged 5–14; M = 8.88; 87 % minority; 53 % male) who attended a community based summer program. Consistent with expectation, reactive aggression was positively associated with child depressive symptoms whereas proactive aggression was negatively associated with depressive symptoms. Further, the interaction between intimate exchange and reactive aggression was associated with child depressive symptoms. Specifically, the association between reactive aggression and depressive symptoms was weaker when levels of intimate exchange were high. Thus, the current study suggests that close peer relationships may help to buffer the link between reactively aggressive behavior and depressive symptoms. Implications for findings include the need to target friendships to help prevent depressive symptoms for reactively aggressive youth. 相似文献
932.
Michael D. Robinson Konrad Bresin 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2014,36(2):201-210
A review of the literature suggests that higher levels of psychopathy may be linked to less effective behavioral control. However, several commentators have urged caution in making statements of this type in the absence of direct evidence. In two studies (total N?=?142), moment-to-moment accuracy in a motor control task was examined as a function of dimensional variations in psychopathy in an undergraduate population. As hypothesized, motor control was distinctively worse at higher levels of psychopathy relative to lower levels, both as a function of primary and secondary psychopathy and particularly their shared variance. These novel findings provide support for the idea that motor control systematically varies by psychopathy, in a basic manner, consistent with views of psychopathy emphasizing lesser control. 相似文献
933.
Jeffrey Michael Chan Lindsay Lambdin Kimberly Graham Christina Fragale Tonya Davis 《Journal of Behavioral Education》2014,23(2):247-257
Individuals with developmental disabilities have limited opportunities to participate in leisure activities, frequently due to lack of skills. The purpose of the current study was to teach three adults diagnosed with mild intellectual disability to use an iPad in the context of playing the video game Angry Birds. We used an adapted multiple baseline across participants research design to teach the task, which included opening the cover of the iPad, unlocking the device, opening Angry Birds, playing the game, and putting the iPad away. Instruction took place during breaks in a private room at a sheltered workshop. During intervention, participants had access to an activity schedule to aid in completion of the task. In some cases, extra prompts were provided to increase attention to the activity schedule. All three participants showed improvement in the target skill with intervention and gains were sustained during the maintenance phase. 相似文献
934.
Desiree R. Azizoddin Geraldine Zamora-Racaza Sarah R. Ormseth Lekeisha A. Sumner Chelsie Cost Julia R. Ayeroff Michael H. Weisman Perry M. Nicassio 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2017,24(3-4):302-315
Our analyses examined whether reserve capacity factors would explain the relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and symptoms of depression/anxiety in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus (SLE). We assessed disease activity, depression/anxiety symptoms, and intrapersonal and interpersonal reserve capacity measures in 128 patients with SLE. Multiple meditational analyses revealed that intrapersonal and interpersonal psychosocial aspects of reserve capacity fully mediated the relationship between SES and depression/anxiety. Lower SES was indirectly associated with higher symptoms of depression and anxiety through the effects of psychosocial resilience. Interventions aimed at improving modifiable reserve capacity variables, such as self-esteem and optimism, may improve anxious/depressive symptomatology in patients with SLE. 相似文献
935.
The current study examines the influence of major life events compared to personality traits on the stability of affective well-being (AWB). It is shown how the decomposition of autocovariances can be used to estimate the degree of stability that is due to life events and personality traits. The approach is illustrated by an analysis of data from a nationally representative Australian sample with four consecutive waves of measurement. The autocovariances of positive and negative mood served as indicators of the stability of AWB. Big five personality traits as well as 21 major life events were used as predictor variables in a path analysis. In comparison to previous studies, this allowed to directly compare the effects of multiple events to the influence of personality traits in a longitudinal design. Results indicated medium stability coefficients for AWB. Ca. 10 % of the autocovariances could be accounted for by life events and about 20 % by personality traits. Both types of predictors together accounted for about 30 %. Among personality traits, emotional stability followed by extraversion had the highest effects. Among life events “financial worsening” and “serious personal injury/illness” had the highest effects. Additionally, life events were significantly correlated with personality traits as well as with other life events. In a next step, it might be interesting to compare the influence of Big Five personality traits and stable life circumstances on the stability of the cognitive well-being component, using a similar statistical procedure. Also, as the number of variables in large panel studies is limited, results should be complemented by more in-depth studies, favourably using multi-method approaches. 相似文献
936.
Women’s sexual desire, agency, and activity have long been stigmatized. In contemporary times, however, adult novelty parties, or gatherings where women can learn about and purchase sex toys or other sensual aids in a group setting, are widespread. The current pervasiveness of novelty parties may be indicative of greater acceptance of women’s sexuality in contemporary Western society. The goal of the present study is to determine whether young people are more accepting of women’s sexuality and desire via their evaluations of women who attend novelty parties relative to more traditional kitchen product parties. In two experiments, U.S. college students read either a novelty party catalog or a kitchen party catalog, and they evaluated hypothetical female attendees of their respective party catalog type across ten total domains. In Experiment 1 (n = 205), novelty party attendees were rated as more vivacious, less traditional, and more insecure than were kitchen party attendees. In Experiment 2 (n = 211), women were harsher than men on novelty party attendees compared to kitchen party attendees in most domains, and men rated novelty party attendees as more masculine than kitchen party attendees, whereas women rated novelty party attendees as less masculine and feminine than kitchen party attendees. Applied and social implications of the present results are discussed, highlighting ways to improve perceptions of women’s sexual desires and pleasure. 相似文献
937.
Do the production and interpretation of patterns of plural forms in noun-noun compounds reveal the workings of innate constraints that govern morphological processing? The results of previous studies on compounding have been taken to support a number of important theoretical claims: first, that there are fundamental differences in the way that children and adults learn and process regular and irregular plurals, second, that these differences reflect formal constraints that govern the way the way regular and irregular plurals are processed in language, and third, that these constraints are unlikely to be the product of learning. In a series of seven experiments, we critically assess the evidence that is cited in support of these arguments. The results of our experiments provide little support for the idea that substantively different factors govern the patterns of acquisition, production and interpretation patterns of regular and irregular plural forms in compounds. Once frequency differences between regular and irregular plurals are accounted for, we find no evidence of any qualitative difference in the patterns of interpretation and production of regular and irregular plural nouns in compounds, in either adults or children. Accordingly, we suggest that the pattern of acquisition of both regular and irregular plurals in compounds is consistent with a simple account, in which children learn the conventions that govern plural compounding using evidence that is readily available in the distribution patterns of adult speech. 相似文献
938.
939.
Cultural differences in process-focused versus person-focused themes were examined using both cultural artifacts (Study 1) and self-reports (Study 2). In Study 1, the contents of Chinese and American graduation and encouragement cards were analyzed for their relative emphasis on person- versus process-focused themes. Person-focused themes center on recipients' stable traits and abilities, and their emotional well-being. In contrast, process-focused themes dwell on the recipients' hard work and effort, and emphasize the importance of continued self-improvement and growth. Messages on Chinese cards were significantly more process than person focused. The reverse was true of American cards. Chinese cards also contained more process-focused (e.g., winding roads) than person-focused images (e.g., student standing on a pedestal). American cards contained more person-focused than process-focused images, although this difference was significant only among encouragement cards. In Study 2, we presented Chinese and American participants with graduation card messages differing in focus. Chinese parents indicated that they would be more likely to select and Chinese students indicated that they would be more likely to receive process- than person-themed graduation card messages. American parents and students showed no effects of message focus. The findings illustrate how cultural beliefs are reflected in cultural artifacts and personal preferences. 相似文献
940.
Beran MJ 《Animal cognition》2011,14(5):637-645
The isolation effect (or von Restorff effect) occurs when one item in a to-be-remembered list is distinctive from all remaining
items, and memory for that item is enhanced. Four chimpanzees were presented with a serial list of four photographs. In the
homogeneous condition, all list items were from the same semantic category (e.g., four fruits). In the isolate condition,
three items were from the same category, but the fourth item (the isolate) was from a different category (e.g., three fruits
and one toy). Then, two photographs were presented, and the chimpanzees had to select the one that was from the list. Two
of four chimpanzees were significantly more likely to select a correct isolate item than an item from the same list position
in the homogeneous condition for at least some list positions. This facilitation in performance was for isolate items only,
as presenting an isolate item in a list did not facilitate greater recognition of other list items compared to the homogeneous
condition. These results indicated that some chimpanzees perceived the semantic categories of the photographs, and categorization
of photographs led to the isolation effect. Thus, chimpanzees may share with humans some aspects of memory organization that
involve spontaneously categorizing visual stimuli and recognizing categorically unique stimuli. 相似文献