首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   46721篇
  免费   1755篇
  国内免费   17篇
  2020年   502篇
  2019年   584篇
  2018年   839篇
  2017年   772篇
  2016年   904篇
  2015年   617篇
  2014年   794篇
  2013年   4546篇
  2012年   1358篇
  2011年   1378篇
  2010年   937篇
  2009年   936篇
  2008年   1371篇
  2007年   1255篇
  2006年   1159篇
  2005年   986篇
  2004年   993篇
  2003年   995篇
  2002年   944篇
  2001年   1330篇
  2000年   1311篇
  1999年   1020篇
  1998年   547篇
  1997年   481篇
  1996年   583篇
  1995年   592篇
  1994年   571篇
  1993年   540篇
  1992年   994篇
  1991年   893篇
  1990年   894篇
  1989年   791篇
  1988年   803篇
  1987年   780篇
  1986年   766篇
  1985年   763篇
  1984年   705篇
  1983年   582篇
  1982年   482篇
  1981年   510篇
  1980年   470篇
  1979年   661篇
  1978年   520篇
  1976年   470篇
  1975年   580篇
  1974年   587篇
  1973年   634篇
  1972年   590篇
  1971年   530篇
  1968年   545篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Informed by theory and research on attributions and narrative persuasion, we compared the effectiveness of narrative and nonnarrative messages in changing attributions of responsibility for causes and solutions related to obesity in the United States. We randomly assigned 500 adults to view one of three messages (narrative, evidence, and a hybrid of the two) emphasizing environmental causes of obesity, or a no‐exposure control condition. The narrative condition increased the belief that societal actors (government, employers) are responsible for addressing obesity, but only among liberals. This pattern of results was partially explained by the success of the narrative condition in reducing reactive counterarguing, relative to the evidence condition, among liberals. We conclude with theoretical and practical implications.  相似文献   
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
In Experiment 1, rats were trained on a discrimination between rubber- and sandpaper-covered arms of a maze after one group had been pre-exposed to these intra-maze cues. Pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made easier by adding further discriminative stimuli, when it now significantly retarded learning. In Experiment 2, rats were trained on an extra-maze spatial discrimination, again after one group, but not another, had been pre-exposed to the extra-maze landmarks. Here too, pre-exposure facilitated subsequent discrimination learning, unless the discrimination was made substantially easier by arranging that the two arms between which rats had to choose were always separated by 135°. The results of both experiments can be explained by supposing that perceptual learning depends on the presence of features common to S+ and S-.  相似文献   
78.
79.
A series of stereograms are presented which demonstrate that texture boundaries can strongly influence the perception of discontinuities between neighbouring three-dimensional (3-D) surfaces portrayed by means of stereo cues. In these demonstration figures, no stereo information is available in the immediate vicinity of the boundary between the two 3-D stereo surfaces because all texture in that region is removed in one eye's view. On the other hand, various forms of texture boundary information are provided in the resulting monocular region. This stimulus paradigm is used to explore the question: what influence does texture boundary information have on the nature of the perceived 3-D surface that is interpolated between two stimulus regions which carry stereo cues? It is shown that if a clear-cut texture boundary is present in the monocular region then this is used by the human visual system to fix the perceived location of 3-D crease and step surface discontinuities between the stereo regions. Collett (1985) explored this issue with a similar methodology and reported weak and unreliable assistance from monocular texture boundaries in helping shape 3-D stereo surface discontinuities. The strong and robust phenomena demonstrated here seem to rely on two main differences between the present stimuli and those of Collett. In the present stimuli, figurally continuous textures containing strong texture boundaries are used, together with a technique for minimising the complications, including binocular rivalry, that arise from the borders of the stimulus regions present in only one half of each stereogram.  相似文献   
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号