全文获取类型
收费全文 | 42015篇 |
免费 | 1786篇 |
国内免费 | 30篇 |
专业分类
43831篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 535篇 |
2019年 | 662篇 |
2018年 | 894篇 |
2017年 | 838篇 |
2016年 | 976篇 |
2015年 | 651篇 |
2014年 | 814篇 |
2013年 | 3614篇 |
2012年 | 1485篇 |
2011年 | 1478篇 |
2010年 | 958篇 |
2009年 | 1029篇 |
2008年 | 1356篇 |
2007年 | 1358篇 |
2006年 | 1208篇 |
2005年 | 1054篇 |
2004年 | 1080篇 |
2003年 | 986篇 |
2002年 | 989篇 |
2001年 | 1216篇 |
2000年 | 1240篇 |
1999年 | 940篇 |
1998年 | 500篇 |
1997年 | 443篇 |
1996年 | 385篇 |
1995年 | 443篇 |
1994年 | 402篇 |
1993年 | 410篇 |
1992年 | 747篇 |
1991年 | 691篇 |
1990年 | 666篇 |
1989年 | 641篇 |
1988年 | 645篇 |
1987年 | 580篇 |
1986年 | 561篇 |
1985年 | 619篇 |
1984年 | 545篇 |
1983年 | 487篇 |
1982年 | 410篇 |
1979年 | 571篇 |
1978年 | 426篇 |
1977年 | 384篇 |
1976年 | 401篇 |
1975年 | 422篇 |
1974年 | 504篇 |
1973年 | 515篇 |
1972年 | 433篇 |
1971年 | 373篇 |
1968年 | 432篇 |
1967年 | 382篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
The development of a 42-item measure of a number of personality features derived from the theory of psychological reversals is reported. These personality features are (a) the extent to which a person is serious-minded, (b) the extent to which a person plans ahead and organizes himself in the pursuit of goals, and (c) the extent to which a person seeks to avoid arousal. These features together are seen to constitute a personality trait which is described as telic dominance. Data concerning the test-retest reliability, criterion-related and construct validity of the Telic Dominance Scale are presented, together with the scale and scoring key. 相似文献
992.
Golden CJ 《Journal of personality assessment》1978,42(2):167-170
Cross-cultural personality research has generated a great amount of data on individual difference patterns in diverse cultures. One of the major instruments used in this research has been Cattell's 16PF. A major question in this research is whether the underlying personality structure is equivalent for different cultures. The present study evaluated the second order factor structure of the 16PF in 101 subjects of European ancestry and 117 subjects of Japanese ancestry. The factor structure for the Japanese was significantly different from that of the caucasian group. The caucasian results did not differ from those reported by Cattell and his associates. The implications of these results for personality theory and for cross-cultural evaluation were briefly discussed. 相似文献
993.
Kelly D. Brownell Carol L. Heckerman Robert J. Westlake Steven C. Hayes Peter M. Monti 《Behaviour research and therapy》1978,16(5):323-333
To evaluate the influence of spouse co-operativeness and couples training in the treatment of obesity, 29 obese men and women were assigned to three experimental conditions: (1) Co-operative spouse-couples training: subjects attended all meetings with spouses. Spouses were trained in modeling, monitoring, and reinforcement techniques; (2) Co-operative spouse-subject alone: subjects attended meetings alone even though their partners had agreed to become involved in treatment; (3) Non-cooperative spouse: subjects had spouses refusing to participate in the program, and attended sessions alone. At the 3-month and 6-month maintenance assessments. subjects in the spouse training condition lost significantly more weight than subjects in the other two conditions. Weight losses compared favorably to those of any controlled study with subjects in the couples training group averaging nearly 30 lbs lost after 8 months of treatment. In the absence of spouse training, subjects with co-operative spouses did no better than subjects with non-co-operative spouses. The findings suggest that spouse training may have a potent facilitative effect in weight reduction, and that this effect may promote long-term maintenance of weight loss. 相似文献
994.
995.
996.
Roy H. Marlowe Charles H. Madsen Charles E. Bowen Robert C. Reardon Patrick E. Logue 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1978,11(1):53-66
This experiment was designed to determine the relative effectiveness of teacher and counselling approaches in the reduction of disruptive or inappropriate classroom behavior. Inappropriate classroom behavior frequencies of 12 academically low achieving, seventh-grade, black male students, with a reported high rate of inappropriate classroom behavior, were recorded. Three groups, with nearly equal mean inappropriate behaviors, were randomly assigned to one of three treatment conditions: behavioral counselling, client-centered counselling, or no counselling. Each counselling group received fifteen 30-minute counselling sessions, at a rate of two to three times a week. In addition to counselling, all students subsequently received teacher approval within the classroom. Results indicated that the teacher was able to reduce inappropriate behavior more than any counselling group. There were also indications that behavioral counselling, but not client-centered counselling, was moderately helpful in reducing inappropriate classroom behavior. 相似文献
997.
This paper presents an interactional approach to the problem of dysfunctional silencing in family therapy. Silencing is classified as dysfunctional if it satisfies two conditions: (a) it occurs repeatedly and independently of content, and (b) it functions as negative feedback that limits change in the family system. Dysfunctional silencing is defined as those efforts of one or more family members to limit change by repeatedly blocking the communication of another family member, who in turn colludes by tacitly agreeing to remain silent. The interactional approach presented utilizes conflict-resolution techniques and videotape feedback; it is illustrated by a case example. 相似文献
998.
In a replication and extension of earlier studies by Hermelin & O'Connor, language recoding abilities in autistic, retarded and normal children matched for mental age and digit span, were compared in a verbal recall task. Random word lists, sentences, and anomalous sentences, eight or 12 items in length (for high and low memory span subgroups) were presented and the number of words recalled from each type of input was scored. All low span children recalled sentences better than random lists with normal children superior to retarded and autistic children and the latter group poorer than the retarded group. Autistic children showed a recency effect with both types of input. There were no group differences amongst high span children and sentences were again better recalled than random lists. In Expt II sentences were better recalled than anomalous sentences, with autistic and retarded children equivalent in performance and poorer than normal children. Although low span autistic children were clearly deficient in recall of sentence material when compared with the two control groups, the effect of conditions showed that they were able to use structure to improve recall. Since high span autistic children did not perform differently from controls it is suggested that results from this kind of study may not be generalizable, and that claims for a specific coding deficit in autistic children need further substantiation. 相似文献
999.
C H Vanderwolf B Kolb R K Cooley 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1978,92(1):156-175
After surgical removal of the neocortex and hippocampal formation, rats retained most of the movement patterns of locomotion, climbing, grooming, feeding, and fighting. However, forepaw immobility during swimming was abolished. Feeding behavior was suppressed temporarily but recovered partially. The distinctive postures of sleep and walking and a circadian rhythm of motor activity were retained. However, behaviors were often not performed at the appropriate time and place. The normal sequence of grooming behavior was disrupted; food hoarding and social behavior were essentially abolished. Removal of the neocortex alone had much the same effect as removal of neocortex and hippocampus together. Removal of hippocampus alone produced only a mild disruption of behavior. It is suggested that ascending nonspecific projections to the cerebral cortex play an important role in the moment-to-moment control of behavior but are not essential for the sleep-waking cycle. 相似文献
1000.
C G Gross 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1978,92(6):1095-1109
Ablation of inferior temporal cortex in the rhesus monkey produces a visual discrimination learning deficit. The severity of this deficit has often been found to be a function of task difficulty. This report concerns a type of visual discrimination problem that, although difficult, is not sensitive to inferior temporal lesions. Monkeys with anterior, posterior, and complete inferior temporal lesions were repeatedly unimpaired or only slightly impaired in learning to discriminate a pattern from the same pattern rotated 90 degrees or 180 degrees; yet they were very severely impaired in learning equally or more difficult discriminations of two different patterns. This demonstration that discrimination of orientation of patterns is relatively spared after inferior temporal lesions helps specify the pattern-recognition processes that require inferior temporal cortex. 相似文献