全文获取类型
收费全文 | 65357篇 |
免费 | 1123篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
66485篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 929篇 |
2019年 | 1165篇 |
2018年 | 1334篇 |
2017年 | 1321篇 |
2016年 | 1636篇 |
2015年 | 1163篇 |
2014年 | 1222篇 |
2013年 | 5711篇 |
2012年 | 2120篇 |
2011年 | 2281篇 |
2010年 | 1429篇 |
2009年 | 1427篇 |
2008年 | 2134篇 |
2007年 | 2072篇 |
2006年 | 1841篇 |
2005年 | 1648篇 |
2004年 | 1660篇 |
2003年 | 1505篇 |
2002年 | 1524篇 |
2001年 | 1923篇 |
2000年 | 1838篇 |
1999年 | 1429篇 |
1998年 | 838篇 |
1997年 | 717篇 |
1996年 | 616篇 |
1995年 | 652篇 |
1994年 | 632篇 |
1993年 | 636篇 |
1992年 | 1120篇 |
1991年 | 1049篇 |
1990年 | 1031篇 |
1989年 | 944篇 |
1988年 | 919篇 |
1987年 | 898篇 |
1986年 | 887篇 |
1985年 | 947篇 |
1984年 | 778篇 |
1983年 | 666篇 |
1982年 | 570篇 |
1981年 | 550篇 |
1980年 | 534篇 |
1979年 | 758篇 |
1978年 | 588篇 |
1976年 | 535篇 |
1975年 | 654篇 |
1974年 | 697篇 |
1973年 | 652篇 |
1972年 | 571篇 |
1968年 | 560篇 |
1967年 | 557篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
Claude A. M. Valiquette Alain D. Lesage Mireille Cyr Jean Toupin 《Behavior research methods》1994,26(1):60-61
This short paper proposes a general computing strategy to compute Kappa coefficients using the SPSS MATRIX routine. The method is based on the following rationale. If the contingency table is considered as a square matrix, then the observed proportions of agreement lie in the main diagonal’s cells, and their sum equals the trace of the matrix, whereas the proportions of agreement expected by chance are the joint product of marginals. The generalization to weighted kappa, which requires an additional square matrix of disagreement weights, both matrices having the same order, becomes possible by the use of the Hadamard product-that is, the elementwise direct product of two matrices. 相似文献
822.
Michael Borenstein 《Behavior research methods》1994,26(3):352-365
This paper describes a method for generating sample survival distributions from a hypothetical population, as would be required for running Monte Carlo simulations. The method is based on the concept of a quincunx. Cases are entered into a life table and allowed to drop out or die during each interval with probabilities that mirror the hypothetical population. By repeating this process many times and tracking the results, the researcher is able to study the sampling distribution of effect size indices and test statistics, and can generate empirical estimates of power and precision for planned studies. Unlike other methods that are commonly used for this purpose, the model proposed here allows the researcher to define a population in which the hazard rates and/or attrition rates vary substantially from one time point to the next, as may be the case in clinical trials or studies of processing times. The method requires less than 100 lines of code and runs some 10,000 simulations per hour on a microcomputer. 相似文献
823.
Tom C. A. Freeman Mike G. Harris P. A. Tyler 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1994,55(6):689-699
Estimates of temporal proximity (sometimes called time-to-collision) from random-dot flow patterns are shown to be based upon retinal speed, rather than upon changes in dot density. Neither the spatial nor the temporal gradient of motion is essential to the task, but estimates can be made from either alone. Performance is unaffected by the addition of rotational motion, suggesting that observers axe capable of extracting the radial component of motion, which contains all the relevant information, from complex stimuli. 相似文献
824.
In previous research, replicated here, we found that some object recognition processes influence figure-ground organization. We have proposed that these object recognition processes operate on edges (or contours)detected early in visual processing, rather than on regions. Consistent with this proposal, influences from object recognition on figure-ground organization were previously observed in both pictures and stereograms depicting regions of different luminance, but not in randomdot stereograms, where edges arise late in processing (Peterson & Gibson, 1993). In the present experiments, we examined whether or not two other types of contours—outlines and subjective contours—enable object recognition influences on figure-ground organization. For both types of contours we observed a pattern of effects similar to that originally obtained with luminance edges. The results of these experiments are valuable for distinguishing between alternative views of the mechanisms mediating object recognition influences on figure-ground organization. In addition, in both Experiments 1 and 2, fixated regions were seen as figure longer than nonfixated regions, suggesting that fixation location must be included among the variables relevant to figure-ground organization. 相似文献
825.
Jeffrey T. Andre Richard A. Tyrrell Herschel W. Leibowitz Mary E. Nicholson Minqi Wang 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1994,56(3):261-267
Contrast sensitivity was measured for 12 healthy young males while sober, after ingestion of an alcohol placebo, and after ingestion of alcohol (95% grain alcohol; mean estimated blood alcohol level = .088%). Observations were made for both stationary gratings and gratings that traveled through a circular path and required pursuit eye movements. The significant alcohol-related reduction in contrast sensitivity was 2.6 times greater for moving (.29-log-unit reduction) than for stationary gratings (.11-log-unit reduction). The loss in contrast sensitivity for the moving gratings of high spatial frequency (12 cpd) was particularly severe (.37 log unit). Estimated blood alcohol level was correlated with the loss in contrast sensitivity for moving gratings (r = .61), but not with the loss for stationary gratings. Estimated blood alcohol level was strongly correlated with the difference between the loss in contrast sensitivity to moving and stationary gratings (r = .75). These results are consistent with reports that alcohol consumption degrades the ability to make pursuit eye movements. Subjects’ perceived intoxication level was not a reliable predictor of any index of visual performance. 相似文献
826.
The present study investigated 2-month-olds’ abilities to discriminate allophonic differences that are potentially useful in segmenting fluent speech. Experiment 1 investigated infants’ sensitivity to the kind of distinction that may signal the presence or absence of a word boundary. When tested with the high-amplitude sucking procedure, infants discriminated pairs of items, such as “nitrate” versus “night rate” and “nikrate” versus “nike rate.” By greatly reducing the potential contribution of prosodic differences to these contrasts, Experiment 2 evaluated whether the allophonic differences for /t/ and /r/ were sufficient for infants to distinguish the “nitrate” versus “night rate” pair. Infants distinguished “nitrate” from a cross-spliced version of “night rate,” which differed only in the allophones for /t/ and /r/ that it included. Thus, infants appear to possess one of the prerequisite capacities (i.e., the ability to discriminate allophonic distinctions) necessary to use allophonic information in segmenting fluent speech. 相似文献
827.
828.
Pastoral Psychology - 相似文献
829.
830.