全文获取类型
收费全文 | 69601篇 |
免费 | 1033篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
70640篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 571篇 |
2019年 | 650篇 |
2018年 | 4089篇 |
2017年 | 3432篇 |
2016年 | 3049篇 |
2015年 | 901篇 |
2014年 | 933篇 |
2013年 | 3855篇 |
2012年 | 2095篇 |
2011年 | 3733篇 |
2010年 | 3371篇 |
2009年 | 2357篇 |
2008年 | 2985篇 |
2007年 | 3403篇 |
2006年 | 1275篇 |
2005年 | 1347篇 |
2004年 | 1303篇 |
2003年 | 1144篇 |
2002年 | 1111篇 |
2001年 | 1460篇 |
2000年 | 1447篇 |
1999年 | 1071篇 |
1998年 | 587篇 |
1997年 | 502篇 |
1992年 | 975篇 |
1991年 | 908篇 |
1990年 | 926篇 |
1989年 | 814篇 |
1988年 | 809篇 |
1987年 | 782篇 |
1986年 | 793篇 |
1985年 | 883篇 |
1984年 | 698篇 |
1983年 | 635篇 |
1982年 | 469篇 |
1981年 | 475篇 |
1979年 | 784篇 |
1978年 | 523篇 |
1977年 | 498篇 |
1976年 | 499篇 |
1975年 | 642篇 |
1974年 | 740篇 |
1973年 | 767篇 |
1972年 | 653篇 |
1971年 | 619篇 |
1970年 | 587篇 |
1969年 | 579篇 |
1968年 | 769篇 |
1967年 | 675篇 |
1966年 | 598篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Using a multi-trial, free-recall paradigm (Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; Lezak, 1983), the acquisition and retention by learning disabled (LD) young adults with a Halstead Impairment Index less than or equal to 0.3 (low LD) or greater than or equal to 0.4 (high LD) was compared with that of nondisabled and head-injured (HI) young adults. Significant group differences, particularly during the last three acquisition trials, confirmed the effectiveness of this method in revealing subtle learning and retention deficits. Analyses of Trial 1 recall showed group differences in the prerecency but not the recency portion of the serial position curve, thus implicating long-term but not short-term memory differences between the groups. Analyses of pair frequency (Sternberg & Tulving, 1977) data showed that the three clinical groups had difficulty organizing the to-be-learned material for effective retrieval. Following an interpolated task, retention was poorer for the clinical groups than for the nondisabled and varied inversely with the degree of neuropsychological impairment. Finally, during retention, the nondisabled and low LD groups retained and used the list organization established during acquisition whereas the high LD and HI groups did not. 相似文献
22.
Subjects were presented with word pairs (e.g., bed-mattress) and timed as they decided whether one word named part of the item named by the other word. Yes responses were facilitated, and no responses were impeded, by relation similarity (i.e., the similarity of the relation between the two stimulus items to the part-whole relation). Item similarity, the similarity of the two stimulus items to each other, had no effect. The results support the view that similarity effects in semantic decision tasks are due to the comparison of elements of the relation between the two stimulus words against relation elements that serve as criteria for the target relation that the subject has been asked to identify. 相似文献
23.
Thirty pediatricians and 30 parents were interviewed on (a) how and when children develop concepts about death, (b) children's emotional responses to the thought of death, (c) the possibility of dialogue between adults and children about death, and (d) differences in healthy and seriously ill children's conceptualization of death. Pediatricians were no different than parents in their opinions. Moreover, in some cases, less educated parents and older doctors tended to hold opinions that were even farther from reality. Overall, the results suggest that children, healthy or sick, are left alone with the problem of death and are not helped either by their parents or by their doctors. 相似文献
24.
25.
Training in systemic therapy necessitates a structured approach to teaching systemic thinking. The Systemic Therapy Sessions Summary Form, a record-keeping method, is presented as a tool for enhancing training in systemic therapy. The purpose of this form is to provide the structure for the trainee to develop circular hypotheses, to connect reflexively the assessment and intervention components of therapy, and to maintain a sense of the evolving nature of therapy across sessions. A case example illustrates the use of this record-keeping format. 相似文献
26.
27.
Amnesia as a consequence of male rape: a case report 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
28.
Wolfgang Hell Gerd Gigerenzer Siegfried Gauggel Maria Mall Michael Müller 《Memory & cognition》1988,16(6):533-538
If subjects are asked to recollect a former response after having been informed about the correct response, their recollection tends to approach the correct response. This effect has been termedhindsight bias. We studied hindsight bias in an experiment requiring numerical responses to almanac-type questions for physical quantities. We varied (1) the time at which the correct information was provided, (2) the encoding of the original responses by asking/not asking subjects to give a reason for the respective response, and (3) the motivation to recall correctly. We found that hindsight is less biased if reasons are given and if the correct information is provided at an earlier time. Motivation had only interactive effects: (1) With high motivation to recall correctly, the time the correct information was provided had no influence. (2) With reasons given, the variation of motivation showed no effect. These results rule out purely motivational and purely automatic explanations. 相似文献
29.
Short-term memory for Chinese characters and radicals 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Short-term memory for Chinese radicals and characters, varying in orthographic complexity, frequency, and—for radicals—intercharacter frequency (the number of compound characters that contain the radical), was studied using an immediate free-recall task. When radicals or characters are relatively frequent, so that their pronunciations are well known by literate Chinese, they seem to be maintained in verbal form in short-term memory. For these stimuli, intercharacter frequency and complexity have relatively small influences on memory span. Stimuli low in frequency, with pronunciations that are not apt to be known, seem to be maintained in visual form in short-term memory. Memory span is much smaller for these stimuli and is influenced by both intercharacter frequency and complexity. Furthermore, short-term memory for relatively high-frequency characters is interfered with more by a verbal than by a visual intervening task, whereas the opposite is true for low-frequency characters. 相似文献
30.
Analogical transfer, problem similarity, and expertise 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
When we encounter a new problem, we are often reminded of similar problems solved earlier and may use the solution procedure from an old problem to solve a new one. Such analogical transfer, however, has been difficult to demonstrate empirically, even within a single experimental session. This article proposes a framework for conceptualizing analogical problem solving that can account for the conflicting findings in the literature. In addition, the framework leads to two predictions concerning the transfer behavior of experts and novices. These predictions concern both positive and negative transfer and are based on the different types of features included in the problem representations of experts and novices. First, when two problems share structural features but not surface features, spontaneous positive transfer should be more likely in experts than in novices. Second, when two problems share surface but not structural features, spontaneous negative transfer should be stronger for novices than for experts. These predictions were supported by the results of three experiments involving college students solving a complex arithmetic word problem. 相似文献