首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   41965篇
  免费   1016篇
  国内免费   9篇
  42990篇
  2021年   210篇
  2020年   360篇
  2019年   395篇
  2018年   3886篇
  2017年   3195篇
  2016年   2715篇
  2015年   595篇
  2014年   588篇
  2013年   2138篇
  2012年   1403篇
  2011年   3160篇
  2010年   3015篇
  2009年   1992篇
  2008年   2462篇
  2007年   2859篇
  2006年   767篇
  2005年   833篇
  2004年   812篇
  2003年   696篇
  2002年   656篇
  2001年   617篇
  2000年   625篇
  1999年   463篇
  1998年   308篇
  1997年   250篇
  1996年   204篇
  1995年   234篇
  1994年   224篇
  1993年   219篇
  1992年   330篇
  1991年   323篇
  1990年   316篇
  1989年   302篇
  1988年   313篇
  1987年   290篇
  1986年   257篇
  1985年   271篇
  1984年   271篇
  1983年   204篇
  1982年   223篇
  1981年   203篇
  1980年   188篇
  1979年   247篇
  1978年   202篇
  1976年   184篇
  1975年   201篇
  1974年   232篇
  1973年   235篇
  1972年   188篇
  1968年   193篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
Although peers' and teachers' evaluations of children's prosocial behavior and peers' sociometric ratings frequently have been used in studies of social development, the validity of young children's ratings of others has been questioned, as has that for teachers' ratings of prosocial behavior. In this study, preschoolers' ratings of peers' sociometric status and prosocial behavior, as well as teachers' ratings of children's prosocial dispositions, were obtained. These were correlated with children's naturally occurring prosocial or social behavior; ratings of prosocial behavior also were correlated with children's prosocial moral reasoning and prosocial self-attributions. Peers' sociometric ratings were positively related to children's sociability whereas prosocial ratings were related to helping (but not sharing) behavior. Teachers' ratings of prosocial behavior were not related to frequency of prosocial behaviors, but were positively related to developmentally mature moral judgments and self-reported motives.  相似文献   
62.
63.
This binational study explored the need importance patterns and involvement in work and family contexts. Results reveal that in the work context, growth needs are most important, followed by affiliative and subsistence needs. In the family context, affiliative needs are most important, followed by subsistence and growth needs. Involvement in work and family contexts was found to be influenced by the need satisfaction potential of the respective contexts. This explained the nature of relationship between work and family involvement. The cross-national generalizability and usefulness of a motivational model for research on involvement in multilife-spheres are discussed.  相似文献   
64.
65.
In two studies of Japanese and Australian university students, we examined the resource allocation rules of equity and equality. In both cultures, subjects were universalistic rather than relativistic in their judgments of fairness and alterability of these rules in the work place. They judged the equity rule to be unalterable by legislation or consensus. However, in both studies, the Japanese perceived equity to be less fair and equality to be less unfair than their Australian counterparts. In addition, study 2 indicated that cross-cultural differences in judgments of fairness were influenced by consideration of need. The age of the worker was a more important determinant of fairness judgments for Japanese than for Australians; the debt of a worker was a more important déterminant for Australians than for Japanese. The results are discussed with regard to the role of culture in conceptions of distributive justice.  相似文献   
66.
The Family System Functioning (FSF) scale is a new instrument for measuring dimensions of the intrafamilial environment thought to be important in recovery from major psychiatric illness. Modest statistical correlations were obtained when FSF ratings of laboratory-based family interactions were compared with researcher-guided therapist ratings of FSF based upon the family's behavior in family therapy sessions during the subsequent month. The data from these two settings provide support for the validity of some of the scales. Because of the modest size of the correlations, however, behavior in the laboratory setting may not always be an accurate indicator of how the family will behave in the early weeks of family therapy.  相似文献   
67.
68.
A precursor of language acquisition in young infants   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
  相似文献   
69.
Two modes of learning for interactive tasks   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
N A Hayes  D E Broadbent 《Cognition》1988,28(3):249-276
  相似文献   
70.
Hindsight bias: An interaction of automatic and motivational factors?   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
If subjects are asked to recollect a former response after having been informed about the correct response, their recollection tends to approach the correct response. This effect has been termedhindsight bias. We studied hindsight bias in an experiment requiring numerical responses to almanac-type questions for physical quantities. We varied (1) the time at which the correct information was provided, (2) the encoding of the original responses by asking/not asking subjects to give a reason for the respective response, and (3) the motivation to recall correctly. We found that hindsight is less biased if reasons are given and if the correct information is provided at an earlier time. Motivation had only interactive effects: (1) With high motivation to recall correctly, the time the correct information was provided had no influence. (2) With reasons given, the variation of motivation showed no effect. These results rule out purely motivational and purely automatic explanations.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号