全文获取类型
收费全文 | 32263篇 |
免费 | 182篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
32446篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 70篇 |
2022年 | 68篇 |
2021年 | 126篇 |
2020年 | 226篇 |
2019年 | 260篇 |
2018年 | 3711篇 |
2017年 | 3037篇 |
2016年 | 2543篇 |
2015年 | 438篇 |
2014年 | 409篇 |
2013年 | 1388篇 |
2012年 | 1110篇 |
2011年 | 2851篇 |
2010年 | 2851篇 |
2009年 | 1831篇 |
2008年 | 2211篇 |
2007年 | 2639篇 |
2006年 | 528篇 |
2005年 | 631篇 |
2004年 | 623篇 |
2003年 | 530篇 |
2002年 | 470篇 |
2001年 | 210篇 |
2000年 | 245篇 |
1999年 | 199篇 |
1998年 | 209篇 |
1997年 | 176篇 |
1996年 | 114篇 |
1995年 | 149篇 |
1994年 | 148篇 |
1993年 | 135篇 |
1992年 | 130篇 |
1991年 | 117篇 |
1990年 | 116篇 |
1989年 | 108篇 |
1988年 | 113篇 |
1987年 | 108篇 |
1986年 | 87篇 |
1985年 | 106篇 |
1984年 | 121篇 |
1983年 | 104篇 |
1982年 | 111篇 |
1981年 | 120篇 |
1980年 | 107篇 |
1979年 | 105篇 |
1978年 | 118篇 |
1977年 | 92篇 |
1976年 | 96篇 |
1975年 | 69篇 |
1974年 | 78篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
202.
Michael Pettit 《Journal of the history of the behavioral sciences》2012,48(3):289-290
203.
204.
205.
Mary M. Mitchell PhD Michael Shayne Gallaway PhD MA Amy M. Millikan MD MPH Michael Bell MD MPH 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2012,42(5):486-494
Suicide is one of the leading causes of death among U.S. Army soldiers. Suicide‐related ideation, which is associated with suicide attempts and suicide, can cause considerable distress. In a sample of 1,663 recently redeployed soldiers, we used factor analysis and structural equation modeling to test the associations between combat exposure, unit cohesion, and their interaction in predicting suicide‐related ideation. We found that combat exposure was a significant risk factor for suicide‐related ideation, while unit cohesion was a significant protective factor. The significant interaction between the two factors indicated that soldiers who experienced greater combat exposure but also had higher levels of unit cohesion had relatively lower levels of suicide‐related ideation. In addition, those who had higher levels of combat exposure and lower unit cohesion were most at risk for suicide‐related ideation. Our findings indicate the importance of unit cohesion in protecting soldiers from suicide‐related ideation and suggest a higher risk group of soldiers who should be targeted for interventions. 相似文献
206.
This article describes a multidimensional, multivariate, and multilevel approach to the assessment of nicotine withdrawal. In this prospective study, 70 adult smokers assigned to an active or placebo nicotine patch condition completed multiple daily assessments using an electronic diary. Average and individual growth curves were estimated for affective and nonaffective withdrawal symptoms. All symptoms but hunger increased significantly on the quit day and remained elevated for three weeks. Variability in symptom experiences across individuals increased from pre- to post-quit. Relations between symptom reports (e.g., negative affect or craving) and episodic events (e.g., stressful events or seeing someone smoke) changed from pre-quit to post-quit. Pre-quit increases in negative affect and quit-day increases in craving were inversely related to abstinence three months after the quit day, suggesting that anticipatory and immediate reactions to quitting influence success. 相似文献
207.
Adults are poor deception detectors when examining lies told by adults, on average. However, there are some adults who are better at detecting lies than others. Children learn to lie at a very young age, a behavior that is socialized by parents. Yet, less is known about the ability to detect children's lies, particularly with regard to individual differences in the ability to detect this deception. The current study explored adult raters' ability to discern honesty in children who lied or told the truth about committing a misdeed. Results showed that adults are no better at detecting children's lies than they are with adult lies. In particular, adults were very poor at identifying children's honest statements. However, individual differences did emerge, suggesting that the ability to detect lying in children might be facilitated by relevant experience working with children. Implications for legal and mental health contexts are discussed. 相似文献
208.
Reijntjes A Stegge H Terwogt MM Kamphuis JH Telch MJ 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(6):877-889
We examined children's behavioral coping in response to an in vivo peer rejection manipulation. Participants (N=186) ranging between 10 and 13 years of age, played a computer game based on the television show Survivor and were randomized to either peer rejection (i.e., being voted out of the game) or non-rejection control. During a five-min. post-feedback waiting period children's use of several behavioral coping strategies was assessed. Rejection elicited a marked shift toward more negative affect, but higher levels of perceived social competence attenuated the negative mood shift. Children higher in depressive symptoms were more likely to engage in passive and avoidant coping behavior. Types of coping were largely unaffected by gender and perceived social competence. Implications are discussed. 相似文献
209.
In four experiments, we examined the effect that presenting a verbal stimulus (viz., an English noun) alongside an abstract visual stimulus (viz., a Chinese character) enhances recognition memory for the abstract visual stimulus. Experiment 1 demonstrated that the character-plus-word combination at both encoding and retrieval results in better recognition than does a character-alone presentation or presenting the combination at encoding only. Experiment 2 demonstrated that presenting the word first and then the character results in better performance than does the opposite order. Experiment 3 showed that the concreteness value of the word, not familiarity, is the critical factor. In Experiment 4, presentation time was varied. More time was needed for liftoff from chance level for the word-character combination than for the character-alone presentation. Together, the results suggest that subjects spontaneously assimilate stimulus and word into a single representation by building asymmetric effortful imagery associations, going from the English word to the Chinese character. 相似文献
210.
Michael A. Westerman 《New Ideas in Psychology》2006,24(3):263-274
In response to points raised by Dawson, Fischer, and Stein; Stam; and Stiles, the author delineates what counts as “good” quantitative research according to his proposal for explicitly interpretive quantitative methods. The key points concern how to employ these methods, not which quantitative methods we should use—“strong” rather than “soft” or vice versa. The author discusses ways in which what others would consider “strong” quantitative procedures can be extremely useful, although he argues that these procedures are interpretive. The reconceptualization of quantitative research offered here also suggests that the distinction between quantitative and qualitative research is much less fundamental than most researchers think. In a consideration of what we can say in general about when to employ quantitative and/or qualitative methods, the author returns to the theme of the limits of possible understanding in psychology. 相似文献