全文获取类型
收费全文 | 53198篇 |
免费 | 616篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
53818篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 411篇 |
2019年 | 500篇 |
2018年 | 3953篇 |
2017年 | 3302篇 |
2016年 | 2874篇 |
2015年 | 743篇 |
2014年 | 742篇 |
2013年 | 3062篇 |
2012年 | 1677篇 |
2011年 | 3390篇 |
2010年 | 3144篇 |
2009年 | 2126篇 |
2008年 | 2614篇 |
2007年 | 3084篇 |
2006年 | 950篇 |
2005年 | 1038篇 |
2004年 | 1006篇 |
2003年 | 906篇 |
2002年 | 876篇 |
2001年 | 943篇 |
2000年 | 942篇 |
1999年 | 726篇 |
1998年 | 423篇 |
1997年 | 364篇 |
1996年 | 331篇 |
1995年 | 343篇 |
1994年 | 322篇 |
1993年 | 310篇 |
1992年 | 563篇 |
1991年 | 552篇 |
1990年 | 533篇 |
1989年 | 501篇 |
1988年 | 522篇 |
1987年 | 481篇 |
1986年 | 468篇 |
1985年 | 491篇 |
1984年 | 424篇 |
1983年 | 370篇 |
1982年 | 314篇 |
1981年 | 321篇 |
1979年 | 449篇 |
1978年 | 361篇 |
1977年 | 299篇 |
1975年 | 342篇 |
1974年 | 431篇 |
1973年 | 423篇 |
1972年 | 320篇 |
1971年 | 317篇 |
1968年 | 366篇 |
1967年 | 318篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
861.
Müller U Liebermann-Finestone DP Carpendale JI Hammond SI Bibok MB 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2012,111(2):331-348
This longitudinal study examined the concurrent and predictive relations between executive function (EF) and theory of mind (ToM) in 82 preschoolers who were assessed when they were 2, 3, and 4 years old. The results showed that the concurrent relation between EF and ToM, after controlling for age, verbal ability, and sex, was significant at 3 and 4 years of age but not at 2 years of age. Hierarchical regression analyses showed that EF at age 2 significantly predicted ToM at age 3 and that EF at age 3 significantly predicted ToM at age 4, over and above the effects of age, verbal ability, and prior performance on ToM tasks. However, ToM at ages 2 and 3 did not explain a significant amount of variance in EF at age 4. Bootstrap procedures revealed that verbal ability at age 3 fully mediated the relation between ToM at age 2 and EF at age 4. 相似文献
862.
Effect of contraction intensity [100%, 75%, 50%, and 25% maximum voluntary contraction (MVC)] and movement velocity [0 degrees (isometric)], 50 degrees, 100 degrees, 200 degrees, and 400 degrees/sec. [isovelocities]) on root mean square amplitude (SEMG-RMS) and median frequency power spectrum (SEMG-MNF) of vastus lateralis (VL) surface electromyography was investigated with ten healthy female university students. Peak torque (PT), mean torque (MT), SEMG-MNF, and SEMG-RMS, analyzed using separate repeated-measures analyses of variance (p < or = .05), indicated: (1) an inverse relation between PT and MT and movement velocity, (2) greater SEMG-MNF values during all isovelocity conditions compared with isometric conditions, with highest values occurring at 50 degrees /sec. and at 100% and 75% MVC, and (3) at all contraction intensities SEMG-RMS values were higher during dynamic movements than isometric movements and highest at 200 degrees /sec. Isovelocity contractions were inferred to facilitate a greater recruitment of fast-twitch fibers (via increased SEMG-MNF), which was intensified at 50 degrees /sec., whereas greater overall muscle activation was found at 200 degrees /sec. 相似文献
863.
SB de Boer FV van Oort MC Donker F Verheij AE Boon 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2012,34(3):415-422
Childhood characteristics are associated with life-course-persistent antisocial behavior in epidemiological studies in general population samples. The present study examines this association in an inpatient sample. The purpose is to identify easily measurable childhood characteristics that may guide choice of treatment for adolescent psychiatric inpatients with severe disruptive behavior. Patients (N?=?203) were divided into two groups with either early-onset (EO) or adolescent-onset (AO) disruptive behavior, based on ages at which professional care was used for disruptive behavior, referral to special education, and criminal offences. Both groups differed on several childhood characteristics. No gender differences in these characteristics were found. Logistic regression analysis indicated that individuals with grade retention in primary school, childhood impulsive behavior, and a history of physical abuse, had the highest probability of being member of the EO group. These characteristics are reasonably easy to identify, likely apply to other clinical samples as well, and may help clinicians to target their treatment. 相似文献
864.
Davis CC Claudius M Palinkas LA Wong JB Leslie LK 《Journal of attention disorders》2012,16(8):675-684
Objective: To examine components of family-centered care in families' stories about treatment decision making for their child with ADHD. Method: Twenty-eight families participated in qualitative interviews that addressed families' perspectives on (a) the treatment decision-making process, (b) the cause and impact of their child's symptoms, and (c) treatment goals and preferences. Results: The majority of families preferred to be primary or shared decision makers regarding treatment decisions. Families' perspectives on the cause of the child's symptoms varied and often were not consistent with a biomedical framework. Families described multiple areas of impairment on child, family relationships, and family functioning. Perspectives toward evidence-based treatments were mixed, with families also expressing interest in and pursuing interventions not delineated in current treatment guidelines. Conclusion: These findings reinforce the importance of eliciting families' perspectives and involving these important stakeholders in shared decision making as critical components of family-centered care for children with ADHD. 相似文献
865.
Mikel Burley 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2012,71(1):21-37
This paper critically discusses D. Z. Phillips’ use of literary works as a resource for philosophical reflection on religion.
Beginning by noting Phillips’ suggestion, made in relation to Waiting for Godot, that the possibilities of meaning that we see in a literary work can reveal something of our own religious sensibility,
I then proceed to show what we learn about Phillips from his readings of certain works by Larkin, Tennyson, and Wharton. Through
exploring alternative possible readings, I argue that, although Phillips’ discussions are of considerable philosophical interest,
they undermine his claim to be deploying a purely contemplative hermeneutical method. 相似文献
866.
John Cattich 《Pastoral Psychology》2012,61(2):179-195
This study explores how parts of the clergy system interact in response to congregational and familial demands. Qualitative
analysis of semi-structured interviews with twenty-four clergy and their spouses identified three clergy system models based
on the spiritual meaning and relational dynamics between the clergy, their spouses, and the congregations: The living sacrifice model emphasizes service to the congregation at the expense of personal and familial needs; the faithful spouse and parent model focuses on family needs over the congregation and practices several disciplines to maintain such a focus; and the peacemaker model seeks as much as possible to satisfy their congregation and their family by intentionally juggling their demands. Results
illustrate how relational processes and spiritual meaning contribute to how clergy systems respond to systemic demands and
pressures. 相似文献
867.
Michelle L. Thomas 《Pastoral Psychology》2012,61(1):99-112
The purpose of this study is to examine the relationships among variables identified in research literature as obstacles and
benefits to interprofessional collaboration-- academic education, interprofessional education, teamwork and communication
skills, and trust---as it relates to the participation of clergypersons in interprofessional collaborative practice. The sample
consisted of ordained Christian clergypersons in Florida who voluntarily completed a survey questionnaire. Standard and stepwise
multiple regression was applied to analyze the data. The model accounts for 25% of the variation of interprofessional collaborative
practice. Results also revealed that the education variables explained significantly 20% of the variance of interprofessional
collaborative practice. 相似文献
868.
Chris Tucker 《Philosophical Studies》2012,157(3):323-340
Does inferential justification require the subject to be aware that her premises support her conclusion? Externalists tend to answer “no” and internalists tend to answer “yes”. In fact, internalists often hold the strong higher-level requirement that an argument justifies its conclusion only if the subject justifiably believes that her premises support her conclusion. I argue for a middle ground. Against most externalists, I argue that inferential justification requires that one be aware that her premises support her conclusion. Against many internalists, I argue that this higher-level awareness needn’t be doxastic or justified. I also argue that the required higher-level awareness needn’t be caused in some appropriate way, e.g. by a reliable or properly functioning faculty. I suspect that this weaker higher-level requirement is overlooked because, at first glance, it seems absurd to allow nondoxastic, unjustified, and unreliably-caused higher-level awareness to contribute to inferential justification. One of the central goals of this paper is to explain how such weak awareness can make an essential contribution to inferential justification. 相似文献
869.
Todd R. Long 《Philosophical Studies》2012,157(2):251-266
Michael Bergmann seeks to motivate his externalist, proper function theory of epistemic justification by providing three objections
to the mentalism and mentalist evidentialism characteristic of nonexternalists such as Richard Feldman and Earl Conee. Bergmann
argues that (i) mentalism is committed to the false thesis that justification depends on mental states; (ii) mentalism is
committed to the false thesis that the epistemic fittingness of an epistemic input to a belief-forming process must be due
to an essential feature of that input, and, relatedly, that mentalist evidentialism is committed to the false thesis that
the epistemic fittingness of doxastic response B to evidence E is an essential property of B–E; and (iii) mentalist evidentialism
is “unmotivated”. I object to each argument. The argument for (i) begs the question. The argument for (ii) suffers from the
fact that mentalist evidentialists are not committed to the consequences claimed for them; nevertheless, I show that there
is, in the neighborhood, a substantive dispute concerning the nature of doxastic epistemic fittingness. That dispute involves
what I call “Necessary Fittingness”, the view that, necessarily, exactly one (at most) doxastic attitude (belief, or disbelief, or suspension of judgment) toward a proposition is epistemically fitting with respect to a person’s total evidence at any time. Reflection on my super-blooper
epistemic design counterexamples to Bergmann’s proper function theory reveals both the plausibility of Necessary Fittingness
and a good reason to deny (iii). Mentalist evidentialism is thus vindicated against the objections. 相似文献
870.
Cultures shape the types of goals that students pursue in the classroom. However, research in achievement goal theory seems
to have neglected this cultural aspect with its exclusive focus on individualistically-based goals such as mastery and performance
goals. This emphasis on mastery and performance goals may reflect Western individualist psychological thinking. Thus, social
goals, which may be more salient in collectivist cultures, are relatively neglected. There is a dearth of studies investigating
the role of social goals in academic motivation, and the few studies that did explore them are somewhat problematic. This
paper reviews research done within the achievement goal theory, considers the need for more studies on social goals, and concludes
with the argument that social goals are important in understanding student motivation especially in collectivist cultures. 相似文献