全文获取类型
收费全文 | 346篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
359篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 18篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 41篇 |
2012年 | 28篇 |
2011年 | 25篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有359条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Haier RJ Jung RE Yeo RA Head K Alkire MT 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2005,5(2):246-251
Response time (RT) generally slows with aging, but the contribution of structural brain changes to this slowing is unknown.
We used voxel-based morphometry (VBM) to determine gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM) brain volumes in 9 middle-aged adults
(38–58 years old) and 9 seniors (66–82 years old). We correlated brain volumes with RT assessed in both a simple visual stimulus-response
task and a visual continuous recognition memory task. No GM correlations with simple RT were significant; there was one WM
correlation in the right fusiform gyrus. In the memory task, faster RT was correlated (p<.05, corrected) with less GM in the globus pallidus, the parahippocampus, and the thalamus for both groups. Several Brodmann areas (BA) differed between
the groups such that in each area, less GM was correlated with slower RTs in the middle-aged group but with faster RTs in the senior group (BAs 19, 37, 46, 9, 8, 6, 13, 10, 41, and 7). The results suggest that individual differences in
specific brain structure volumes should be considered as potential moderating factors in cognitive brain imaging studies. 相似文献
12.
13.
Kang SY Lee KY Park MJ Kim YC Markelonis GJ Oh TH Kim YC 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2003,79(1):11-18
We previously reported that a total methanolic extract of the underground part of Angelica gigas Nakai (Umbelliferae) (here-in-after abbreviated AG) significantly inhibited acetylcholinesterase (AChE) activity. We characterized 12 coumarin derivatives including both decursin and decursinol from extracts of AG. In this study, we evaluated the anti-amnestic activity of decursin, a major coumarin constituent isolated from AG, in vivo using ICR mice with amnesia induced by scopolamine (1 mg/kg body weight, s.c.). Decursin, when administered to mice at 1 and 5 mg/kg body weight i.p., significantly ameliorated scopolamine-induced amnesia as measured in both the passive avoidance test and the Morris water maze test. Moreover, decursin significantly inhibited AChE activity by 34% in the hippocampus of treated mice. These results indicate that decursin may exert anti-amnestic activity in vivo through inhibition of AChE activity in the hippocampus. 相似文献
14.
Carolyn A. Blondin Jeff L. Cochran Eun Jung Oh Cora M. Taylor Robert L. Williams 《Journal of Adult Development》2011,18(4):204-213
Undergraduate students in a Southeastern US University (n = 232) responded to an inventory that included retrospective measures of their parents’ style of parenting (authoritarian
vs. authoritative) and their own childhood psychological tendencies (insecurity vs. confidence), as well as their adult stressors
and political orientation. Authoritative parenting positively correlated with childhood confidence and negatively correlated
with both childhood insecurity and adult stressors. Conversely, authoritarian parenting was positively associated with childhood
insecurity and adult stressors but was not significantly correlated with childhood confidence. For the most part, parenting
styles, early childhood tendencies, and adult stressors were unrelated to adult political ideology, contrary to previous longitudinal
research reporting these connections. 相似文献
15.
Jeremy Bailenson Kayur Patel Alexia Nielsen Ruzena Bajscy Sang-Hack Jung Gregorij Kurillo 《Media Psychology》2013,16(3):354-376
Virtual reality (VR) offers new possibilities for learning, specifically for training individuals to perform physical movements such as physical therapy and exercise. The current article examines two aspects of VR that uniquely contribute to media interactivity: the ability to capture and review physical behavior and the ability to see one's avatar rendered in real time from third person points of view. In two studies, we utilized a state-of-the-art, image-based tele-immersive system, capable of tracking and rendering many degrees of freedom of human motion in real time. In Experiment 1, participants learned better in VR than in a video learning condition according to self-report measures, and the cause of the advantage was seeing one's avatar stereoscopically in the third person. In Experiment 2, we added a virtual mirror in the learning environment to further leverage the ability to see oneself from novel angles in real time. Participants learned better in VR than in video according to objective performance measures. Implications for learning via interactive digital media are discussed. 相似文献
16.
Drawing on uncertainty‐identity theory (Hogg, 2012), we explore the effects of uncertainty concerning a specific social identity on group identification and attitudes toward subgroup integration and separation in South Koreans' nested identity context (N = 148). All variables were measured. Path analysis revealed, as predicted, that superordinate identity uncertainty weakened superordinate identification and subgroup identity uncertainty weakened subgroup identification. We also found that subgroup identity uncertainty strengthened superordinate identification. This effect was stronger for those who perceived their superordinate group prototype and subgroup prototype to be distinct and nonoverlapping. Furthermore, superordinate identity uncertainty decreased reunification intentions by weakening superordinate identification. Subgroup identity uncertainty increased reunification intentions by strengthening superordinate identification only for those who perceived their superordinate group prototype and subgroup prototype to be distinct and nonoverlapping. Implications for uncertainty identity theory and intergroup relations are discussed. 相似文献
17.
18.
关于艰苦奋斗精神内涵的文化释读 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
艰苦奋斗精神已是日常话语的热点高频词汇,但多是以事例和名言的方式阐释其重要性,缺乏内涵层面的本质剖析。本文指证艰苦奋斗精神乃中外先进文化共有的精髓,并将之内涵解构为“合理而现实的目标驱动、资源效用最大化追求、竭心尽力的奋斗毅力”三个核心构成要件;批判了日常语境下的“吃苦光荣论”、“国情需要论”、“节俭悖论”等认知误区;阐释了艰苦奋斗精神与节约型社会建设、与生态消费观的现代关系,将对深入理解社会主义荣辱观和先进文化建设具有理论促进作用。 相似文献
19.
20.
John Jung 《Basic and applied social psychology》2013,35(1-2):57-83
An analysis of social psychological aspects of social support examined several stages of the interaction between provider and recipient. In the first stage, the provider may offer support, respond positively to requests for support, or decline requests for support, whereas the recipient may seek, accept, or reject support. In the second stage, after support is exchanged, attributions about its effect may occur for each member, which in turn affects the extent to which the support is continued and the nature of its effect. Attributional errors about the effect may occur, especially if factors other than support—such as external factors—alter the problem status. The final stage follows the resolution of the problem and may involve reciprocation of support when roles are reversed, although such reciprocity may not be a necessary condition for social support. Possible mechanisms for both positive as well as negative effects of support were postulated. Assessing the effects of support is complicated by factors such as individual differences, the degree of match between social support beliefs held by provider and recipient, type of problem, and the timing of support. 相似文献