全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3219篇 |
免费 | 51篇 |
专业分类
3270篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 44篇 |
2023年 | 49篇 |
2022年 | 46篇 |
2021年 | 53篇 |
2020年 | 82篇 |
2019年 | 63篇 |
2018年 | 127篇 |
2017年 | 124篇 |
2016年 | 121篇 |
2015年 | 94篇 |
2014年 | 97篇 |
2013年 | 347篇 |
2012年 | 194篇 |
2011年 | 148篇 |
2010年 | 103篇 |
2009年 | 99篇 |
2008年 | 127篇 |
2007年 | 148篇 |
2006年 | 87篇 |
2005年 | 89篇 |
2004年 | 80篇 |
2003年 | 66篇 |
2002年 | 63篇 |
2001年 | 65篇 |
2000年 | 62篇 |
1999年 | 46篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 22篇 |
1996年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 31篇 |
1989年 | 27篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 21篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 28篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1975年 | 21篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 19篇 |
1971年 | 17篇 |
1970年 | 19篇 |
1968年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有3270条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Asli?Kesimci F.?Sevin??G?ral Tülin?Gen??zEmail author 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2005,24(1):68-75
The present study aimed to investigate the determinants of stress-related growth. For this aim the associations of gender,
stressfulness of the event, and three coping strategies (problem-oriented, fatalistic, and helplessness) with stress-related
growth were tested by multiple regression analysis. Participants were 132 undergraduate students. Results revealed that females
reported higher levels of stress-related growth than males, and as expected, higher levels of the stressfulness of the event
associated with more stress-related growth. Furthermore, frequent utilization of problem-oriented and fatalistic coping strategies
was associated with higher stress-related growth. These findings were discussed in the light of relevant literature and culture-specific
features.
This work has been supported by the Turkish Academy of Sciences, in the framework of the Young Scientist Award Program (TG-TUBA-GEB>P/2002-l-l
1). 相似文献
92.
Allik J 《Journal of personality disorders》2005,19(3):212-232
In order to generalize the dimensional structure of personality-relatively independent groups of covarying traits-across languages and cultures, a large number of cultures must be studied. Until recently only a few worldwide personality datasets have been available. The first large-scale studies indicate that the pattern of covariation between personality traits is universal and is relatively easily generalizable across languages and cultures. In contrast to the structure of personality, the comparison of the mean trait scores across cultures is much more problematic because cross-cultural differences turned out to be very small in their magnitude, about one-third of the magnitude of individual differences within culture. More integral (e.g., the similarity between personality profiles) or subtle (e.g., the disparity between positively and negatively worded items) measures can reveal more systematic relationships with relevant socioeconomic and geographic variables than the mean scores themselves. Relatively modest sizes of cross-cultural differences in the mean values may imply that a reasonable scalar equivalence can be achieved, and all individuals, irrespective of their language and culture, can be represented in a common metric. 相似文献
93.
Lemasson A Hausberger M Zuberbühler K 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2005,119(2):220-229
Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli) frequently exchange vocalizations, the combined-harmonic calls, with individuals responding to one another's calls. Previous work has shown that these calls can be grouped into several structural variants. Adult females differ in their variant repertoires, which may change during their adult life, particularly after changes in the group composition. Playback of females' currently produced variants triggered vocal responses from other group members, whereas the same females' former, no longer used variants and those of stranger females never did. In contrast, former variants caused long-term cessation of vocal behavior, whereas stranger variants had no effect. Data showed that monkeys were able to distinguish between the different types of variants, indicating that these calls form part of a long-term social memory. 相似文献
94.
The skill of writing numerals necessitates a certain developmental maturity. Studying the age at which children start to develop this skill and assessing problems they encounter while writing numerals should give direction to their training. From 8 schools a total of 267 children, ages 60 to 89 mo., were asked to write the numerals 1 to 9. Analysis showed that 80% of the children wrote most of the numerals correctly, 15% wrote inverted numerals, 2.0% wrote some numerals as letters, and 4% did not write certain numerals at all. In addition, 64% of children ages 60 to 65 mo. gave correct responses, and the perception of correct responses increased across age groups until 95% were correct in the group who were 84 to 89 mo. There was no significant difference between girls and boys in these writing skills. 相似文献
95.
Participants are worse at identifying spatial symbols (arrowheads) while performing spatially compatible manual key presses. The present experiments investigated the generality of this blindness effect to response-compatible stimuli. In Experiment 1 a left key press deteriorated the identification of left-pointing arrows, and a right key press deteriorated the perception of right-pointing arrows, independent of the hands used to press the key. Thus the blindness effect is based on codes of the distal response location rather than on the body-intrinsic anatomical connection of the hands. Experiment 2 extended the blindness effect to verbal responses and written position words (left, right, up, down). Vocalizing a position word blinded to directly compatible position words (e.g., left-left), but not to orthogonally compatible position words (e.g., left-down). This result suggests that the use of identical stimulus-response codes, and not the use of saliency-matching but distinct codes, suffices to produce blindness effects. Finally, Experiment 3 extended the blindness phenomenon beyond the spatial domain by demonstrating blindness between saying color words and perceiving color patches. Altogether, the experiments revealed action-induced blindness to be a phenomenon of broad empirical validity occurring whenever action and perception afford simultaneous access to the same conceptual codes. 相似文献
96.
In the first part of this study, we investigated effects of item and task type on span performance in a group of aphasic individuals with word processing and STM deficits. Group analyses revealed significant effects of item on span performance with span being greater for digits than for words. We also investigated associations between subjects' lexical-semantic and phonological processing abilities and performance on four measures of verbal span (digit and word span, each varied for type of response, verbal vs. pointing) as well as one measure of nonverbal span. We predicted and found that the patterns of association between verbal span tasks and lexical abilities reflected the integrity of language processes and representations deployed in each paradigm used to assess span. Performance on the pointing span task, which engages both lexical-semantic and phonological processes, correlated with measures of both lexical-semantic and phonological abilities. Performance on repetition span, which engages primarily input and output phonological processes, correlated with measures of phonological abilities but not measures of lexical-semantic abilities. However, when partial correlations were performed for two subject groups based on their relative preservation of lexical-semantic ability (less or more than phonological ability), repetition span correlated with lexical-semantic measures only in the subgroup with relatively impaired lexical-semantics. Additionally, performance on the nonverbal span task correlated with measures of phonological abilities, suggesting either a general cognitive deficit affecting verbal and nonverbal STM or possibly, the use of a verbal strategy to perform this task. Our discussion focuses on the interpretation of span measurements in clinical practice and research, as well as the implications of these data for theories of short-term memory and word processing. 相似文献
97.
98.
Hübner M Dreisbach G Haider H Kluwe RH 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2003,29(2):289-297
Endogenously initiated transitions between tasks are associated with inhibition of the attentional set for the task preceding the transition, as demonstrated by slowed reactions to a task most recently switched away from (U. Mayr & S. W. Keele, 2000). Using an altered methodological approach, the authors found that this backward inhibition counteracts perseverative tendencies when switching to a new task in that it selectively reduces interference exerted by the preceding task set. The reduction of interference was dependent on endogenous preparation for the new task and did not occur for unpredictable task switches or for task switches that were precued without information about the identity of the new task. 相似文献
99.
Some well-known geometrical illusions disappear when the eyes are fixating and saccades are suppressed for a period of time. This disappearance is not accompanied by fading due to stabilisation of the retinal image. Any saccade made on purpose restores the illusion immediately. The fixation time after which some illusions disappeared was measured for four illusions and four subjects each. Effects of practice have been observed after measurements were repeated on successive days. Present theories of vision cannot readily explain the effect. 相似文献
100.
Sit-and-wait strategies in dynamic visual search 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The role of memory in visual search has lately become a controversial issue. Horowitz and Wolfe (1998) observed that performance in a visual search task was little affected by whether the stimuli were static or randomly relocated every 111 ms. Because a memory-based mechanism, such as inhibition of return, would be of no use in the dynamic condition, Horowitz and Wolfe concluded that memory is likewise not involved in the static condition. However, Horowitz and Wolfe could not effectively rule out the possibility that observers adopted a different strategy in the dynamic condition than in the static condition. That is, in the dynamic condition observers may have attended to a subregion of the display and waited for the target to appear there (sit-and-wait strategy). This hypothesis is supported by experimental data showing that performance in their dynamic condition does not differ from performance in another dynamic condition in which observers are forced to adopt a sit-and-wait strategy by being presented with a limited region of the display only. 相似文献