首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   17476篇
  免费   850篇
  国内免费   16篇
  2021年   145篇
  2020年   255篇
  2019年   282篇
  2018年   421篇
  2017年   399篇
  2016年   396篇
  2015年   301篇
  2014年   338篇
  2013年   1383篇
  2012年   615篇
  2011年   697篇
  2010年   398篇
  2009年   365篇
  2008年   499篇
  2007年   481篇
  2006年   413篇
  2005年   346篇
  2004年   364篇
  2003年   324篇
  2002年   329篇
  2001年   702篇
  2000年   700篇
  1999年   461篇
  1998年   181篇
  1997年   175篇
  1996年   139篇
  1995年   138篇
  1993年   140篇
  1992年   361篇
  1991年   321篇
  1990年   346篇
  1989年   334篇
  1988年   317篇
  1987年   289篇
  1986年   310篇
  1985年   298篇
  1984年   235篇
  1983年   201篇
  1979年   190篇
  1978年   161篇
  1975年   187篇
  1974年   241篇
  1973年   197篇
  1972年   203篇
  1971年   167篇
  1970年   149篇
  1969年   198篇
  1968年   202篇
  1967年   161篇
  1966年   182篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
Three experiments examined the lateralization of lexical codes in auditory word recognition. In Experiment 1 a word rhyming with a binaurally presented cue word was detected faster when the cue and target were spelled similarly than when they were spelled differently. This orthography effect was larger when the target was presented to the right ear than when it was presented to the left ear. Experiment 2 replicated the interaction between ear of presentation and orthography effect when the cue and target were spoken in different voices. In Experiment 3, subjects made lexical decisions to pairs of stimuli presented to the left or the right ear. Lexical decision times and the amount of facilitation which obtained when the target stimuli were semantically related words did not differ as a function of ear of presentation. The results suggest that the semantic, phonological, and orthographic codes for a word are represented in each hemisphere; however, orthographic and phonological representations are integrated only in the left hemisphere.  相似文献   
902.
Forty right-handed college subjects tapped with and without a verbal task under two instructional conditions (tap as quickly as possible vs. tap as consistently as possible) and two levels of verbal production (silent vs. aloud). The tapping task consisted of the alternate tapping of two keys with the index finger of the left vs. right hands, while the verbal task was anagram solution. Three rate and four variability measures of tapping performance were evaluated in the identification of lateralized interference. The results indicate that reliable lateralized interference, more right-hand than left-hand tapping disruption, was observed only for variability measures under instructions to tap as consistently as possible. Furthermore, only one of these variability measures was sensitive to an increase in lateralized interference produced by verbal production. Because of the limited demonstration of verbal laterality effects with the two-key tapping procedure in this study, conclusions suggest that the simpler manual task of repetitive tapping of one key should be viewed as the method of choice in future dual-task studies.  相似文献   
903.
How much information from experience does a normal adult remember? The “functional information content” of human memory was estimated in several ways. The methods depend on measured rates of input and loss from very long- term memory and on analyses of the informational demands of human memory-based performance. Estimates ranged around 109 bits. It is speculated that the flexible and creative retrieval of facts by humans is a function of a large ratio of “hardware” capacity to functional storage requirements.  相似文献   
904.
Three experiments were conducted to investigate the visual/kinesthetic coding characteristics of two movement informations, i.e., location and distance. The first experiments used matching task procedures, the second experiment used visual augmentation and reduction procedures and the third experiment made use of an ischemic nerve block procedure. The findings' of the first experiment indicated a clear kinesthetic coding for distance information. Results suggest a kinesthetic coding for location cue, with the presence of vision (alone) on both phases of movements improving its accuracy. The second experiment ruled out a visual coding of distance cue and indicated a kinesthetic representation of location cue. Again, simultaneous presence of vision (only at the time of criterion movement) enhanced its performance. The third experiment confirmed a kinesthetic signalling of distance cue. Location cue, in conditions of the third experiment did not get influenced. Probably, muscle receptors in relation with central commands can handle location information. A multiple coding for location information is suggested.  相似文献   
905.
906.
907.
908.
909.
910.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号