首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   37347篇
  免费   1613篇
  国内免费   26篇
  2020年   408篇
  2019年   488篇
  2018年   709篇
  2017年   737篇
  2016年   760篇
  2015年   579篇
  2014年   635篇
  2013年   2984篇
  2012年   1153篇
  2011年   1252篇
  2010年   736篇
  2009年   696篇
  2008年   938篇
  2007年   974篇
  2006年   830篇
  2005年   757篇
  2004年   740篇
  2003年   710篇
  2002年   736篇
  2001年   1365篇
  2000年   1326篇
  1999年   952篇
  1998年   381篇
  1997年   354篇
  1996年   348篇
  1992年   772篇
  1991年   717篇
  1990年   739篇
  1989年   699篇
  1988年   685篇
  1987年   623篇
  1986年   669篇
  1985年   650篇
  1984年   518篇
  1983年   457篇
  1982年   322篇
  1981年   329篇
  1979年   529篇
  1978年   405篇
  1977年   335篇
  1975年   450篇
  1974年   572篇
  1973年   546篇
  1972年   478篇
  1971年   428篇
  1970年   418篇
  1969年   431篇
  1968年   536篇
  1967年   454篇
  1966年   454篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
861.
The guidance movement in Taiwan, only 20 years old, has paralleled ours in developing from vocational-educational advisement toward personal counseling. There is a basic conflict between traditional Chinese culture grounded in Confucian ideals and rules of behavior, and the highly individualized value orientation on which the counseling movement is predicated. This conflict leads Chinese guidance workers to search their tradition for a more appropriate balance between individual freedom and social responsibility. This article is a cross-cultural comparison exploring some critical implications for the value orientation of our Western democratic society and the counseling movement.  相似文献   
862.
This article supplements the ideas on counseling and the social revolution which appeared in the May 1971 P&G—especially regarding the common behavior models of the counselor and the change agent. Because adherence to a model often places a counselor in jeopardy, the authors call on the profession to offer support and leadership on a national level to those counselors who wish to act as change agents.  相似文献   
863.
Three dependent variables, derived from an extended Signal-Detection paradigma, were used in each of 3 experiments: memory performance, confidence-level, and response-bias. Each memory item was first judged by S and then fictitiously by 2 confederates providing different degrees of agreement and disagreement. As compared to agreement moderate disagreement yielded both better recognition performance and, if S's judgements were false, less confidence. Strong disagreement failed to repeat these findings. Balanced agreement/disagreement raised the level of both performance and confidence relative to a situation without information from the group. In all the experiments correct decisions yielded higher confidence than errors. Festinger's theory of social comparison processes accounts for all results in performance, but for explaining the confidence shifts assumptions on ‘internal cues’ should also be incorporated. The response-bias was not affected by social treatment differences, thus supporting the view of some Signal-Detection theorists. Proposals towards a general theory of stimulus processing in social context are outlined and some of its consequences are discussed by taking as examples some conformity experiments.  相似文献   
864.
Birth order: a critical review   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
B N Adams 《Sociometry》1972,35(3):411-439
  相似文献   
865.
In two separate experiments, 40 Ss were presented with recorded sentences during each of which a click occurred. Ss had to depress a key as soon as they heard the click. RTs were f aster when the click was located at the major syntactic break of the sentence compared with RTs to clicks not at a break. This confirmed the hypothesis that processing load is a function of the surface structure of sentences, although the role of minor breaks was not clear. A second finding was that RTs were slower when the click was in the first rather than in the second half of the sentence. This can also be explained in terms of differential processing loads.  相似文献   
866.
867.
868.
869.
A new type of multichannel olfactometer is described, and the results are given for gas chromatographical standardization of this equipment. The olfactometer keeps 24 odorous substances available for presentation, consecutively, two per trial, or combinations of the two. It produces concentrations of odorous gas in the air between 1 × l0?5 and 1 × 10?11 molecules/liter. The principle of construction is gradual dilution of an odorous gas in streaming purified air. The design provides for electronic control of stimulus sequence and response registration via punched tapes.  相似文献   
870.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号