首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   24042篇
  免费   1008篇
  国内免费   13篇
  2020年   289篇
  2019年   297篇
  2018年   484篇
  2017年   435篇
  2016年   458篇
  2015年   304篇
  2014年   446篇
  2013年   1858篇
  2012年   735篇
  2011年   807篇
  2010年   418篇
  2009年   520篇
  2008年   703篇
  2007年   715篇
  2006年   672篇
  2005年   566篇
  2004年   538篇
  2003年   515篇
  2002年   502篇
  2001年   841篇
  2000年   833篇
  1999年   612篇
  1998年   265篇
  1997年   250篇
  1996年   217篇
  1993年   214篇
  1992年   516篇
  1991年   438篇
  1990年   512篇
  1989年   433篇
  1988年   399篇
  1987年   375篇
  1986年   406篇
  1985年   460篇
  1984年   350篇
  1983年   300篇
  1982年   232篇
  1979年   330篇
  1978年   268篇
  1977年   215篇
  1975年   310篇
  1974年   355篇
  1973年   361篇
  1972年   279篇
  1971年   250篇
  1970年   216篇
  1969年   233篇
  1968年   294篇
  1967年   257篇
  1966年   275篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
91.
We examined the functional role of verbalizations in the generalization of self-instructional training with preschoolers. Children learned to overtly self-instruct during classroom work periods prior to covert training. Data were collected on children's acquisition of verbal regulation during training and on overt use of self-instructions in the classroom generalization setting. Results of a multiple baseline design across subjects indicated that treatment effects were evident in the training setting but did not generalize to the classroom until children were emitting overt self-instructions in the classroom itself. The production of self-verbalizations in the generalization setting was related to changes in correct responding, on-task behavior, and efficiency in completing academic work.  相似文献   
92.
Using a multi-trial, free-recall paradigm (Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test; Lezak, 1983), the acquisition and retention by learning disabled (LD) young adults with a Halstead Impairment Index less than or equal to 0.3 (low LD) or greater than or equal to 0.4 (high LD) was compared with that of nondisabled and head-injured (HI) young adults. Significant group differences, particularly during the last three acquisition trials, confirmed the effectiveness of this method in revealing subtle learning and retention deficits. Analyses of Trial 1 recall showed group differences in the prerecency but not the recency portion of the serial position curve, thus implicating long-term but not short-term memory differences between the groups. Analyses of pair frequency (Sternberg & Tulving, 1977) data showed that the three clinical groups had difficulty organizing the to-be-learned material for effective retrieval. Following an interpolated task, retention was poorer for the clinical groups than for the nondisabled and varied inversely with the degree of neuropsychological impairment. Finally, during retention, the nondisabled and low LD groups retained and used the list organization established during acquisition whereas the high LD and HI groups did not.  相似文献   
93.
Tobacco, alcohol, and drug use are problems for American-Indian people. We reviewed these problems and the explanations for them and described a bicultural competence skills approach for preventing substance abuse with American-Indian adolescents. Data from a study of that approach suggest its efficacy with American-Indian youth. At posttest and a 6-month follow-up, American-Indian subjects who received preventive intervention based on bicultural competence skills concepts improved more than did American-Indian subjects in a no-intervention control condition on measures of substance-use knowledge, attitudes, and interactive skills, and on self-reported rates of tobacco, alcohol, and drug use. Our findings have implications for future substance-abuse prevention research with American-Indian people.  相似文献   
94.
In Korea, two number systems are in simultaneous use. One is somewhat irregular (like the English language number system), and the other regular (similar to the Chinese number system). This natural experiment allows a direct test of the hypothesis that structural regularity of the number system facilitates the acquisition of counting. Korean children initially suffer from their numerical ‘bilingualism’: they find it difficult to learn two counting systems at the same time. Yet, after this initial difficulty, they soon master the generative rules which are transparent in the regular system. The cognitive advantage provided by the regular number system then combines with other factors, like intensive teaching and parental pressure, to produce school achievement in mathematics superior to US children's.  相似文献   
95.
An Expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
  相似文献   
96.
97.
98.
99.
100.
The startle probe response: a new measure of emotion?   总被引:17,自引:0,他引:17  
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号