全文获取类型
收费全文 | 535篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
538篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 47篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 37篇 |
2010年 | 17篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 23篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有538条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Gibson BM Wilks TJ Kelly DM 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》2007,121(2):130-144
To navigate efficiently, a traveler must establish a heading using a frame of reference. A large body of evidence has indicated that humans and a variety of nonhuman animals utilize the geometry, or shape, of enclosed spaces as a frame of reference to determine their heading. An important and yet unresolved question is whether shape information from arrays of discrete objects and enclosed environments are represented, and utilized, in the same way. In the present study, rats were presented with a reference memory task in which they had to find water that was hidden in 1 of 4 discrete and unique objects placed at the vertices of a rectangle. The results indicate that rats could utilize both feature and geometry cues to locate the hidden goal. The rats' performance declined during transformation tests using a triangular array, indicating that the rats may have encoded the primary axis of the object array, rather than local cues, to direct their search. 相似文献
102.
The Body Vigilance Scale (BVS) is a measure developed to assess one’s conscious attendance to internal cues. The present report investigated the structure, correlates, and predictive utility of the BVS in nonclinical (N = 442) and anxiety (N = 135) disorder samples. The findings of Study 1 suggest that the BVS is 1-dimensional in a nonclinical sample, and Study 2 replicated the factor structure of the BVS in an anxiety disorder sample. Correlations between the BVS and related (i.e., anxiety sensitivity) and unrelated (i.e., social anxiety) variables were consistent with predictions in both studies. Study 2 also showed that body vigilance is primarily elevated in patients with panic disorder relative to other anxiety disorders. Relative elevations in body vigilance were also observed for patients with hypochondriasis and generalized anxiety disorder. The BVS also demonstrated a specific association with medical utilization and health-related safety-seeking behaviors after controlling for related variables in nonclinical and anxiety disorder samples. The implications of our findings for the nature and measurement of body vigilance as a predictor of health concerns in anxiety disorders are considered. 相似文献
103.
104.
Sibling conflict can rise to the level of a clinical problem. In Phase 1 a lengthy behavioral role-play analog sampling child
reactions to normal sibling conflicts was successfully shortened. In Phase 2 normal children who lacked sibling conflict resolution
skills were randomly assigned to a Training or Measurement Only condition. Training consisted of five clinic sessions focusing
on verbal reasoning, assertiveness, and acceptance skills. Trained children outperformed Measurement Only children on the
shortened role-play test. Parents of trained children, but not untrained children, perceived improved social competence in
the home. The project provides an empirical foundation for future research with aggressive siblings. 相似文献
105.
Pigeons and people select efficient routes when solving a one-way "traveling salesperson" task 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Gibson BM Wasserman EA Kamil AC 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》2007,33(3):244-261
The authors presented people (Experiment 1) and pigeons (Experiments 2 and 3) with a large number of 1-way traveling salesperson problems that consisted of 3, 4, and 5 identical stimuli (nodes) on a computer monitor. The sequence of nodes that each traveler selected was recorded, and the distance of the route was subsequently determined. The routes the pigeons and people selected were reliably more efficient than those used by a Monte Carlo model given the same problems. The pigeons' routes were significantly less efficient than a nearest neighbor model of performance, however. In Experiment 3, pigeons were required to select a route that was within the top 33% of all possible solutions for a given problem. The pigeons' solutions were significantly more efficient than those observed in Experiment 2, in which the behavioral criterion was not imposed. The mechanisms that pigeons and people may have been using to solve the traveling salesperson problems are discussed. 相似文献
106.
Catherine H. Tinsley Jeanne M. Brett 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2001,85(2):37
We propose that managers have norms (standards of appropriate behavior) for resolving conflict, that these norms are culturally based, and that they explain cultural differences in conflict management outcomes. We confirm that the traditionally American norms of discussing parties' interests and synthesizing multiple issues were exhibited more strongly by American managers than by their Hong Kong Chinese counterparts. In addition, we confirm that the traditionally Chinese norms of concern for collective interests and concern for authority appeared more strongly among Hong Kong Chinese managers than among their American counterparts. American managers were more likely than Hong Kong Chinese managers, to resolve a greater number of issues and reach more integrative outcomes, while Hong Kong Chinese managers were more likely to involve higher management in conflict resolution. Culture had a significant effect on whether parties selected an integrative outcome rather than an outcome that involved distribution, compromise, higher management, or no resolution at all. Conflict norms explained the cultural differences that existed between reaching an integrative outcome and reaching an outcome involving distribution, compromise, or higher management; however, conflict norms did not fully explain the cultural differences that existed between reaching an integrative outcome and reaching no resolution. 相似文献
107.
108.
The Psychological Record - Treatment with 8.0 mg/kg fluprazine hydrochloride had no effect on the acquisition of a step-down avoidance response but retarded its extinction. The drug also impaired... 相似文献
109.
110.
Feinstein Robert Gooden Brett A. Wolf Stewart 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1975,10(2):102-111
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science - Repeated EEG and ECG monitoring during sleep was carried out in two subjects with ischemic heart disease and one presumably healthy control. All... 相似文献