首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   508篇
  免费   15篇
  国内免费   1篇
  524篇
  2023年   4篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   10篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   19篇
  2015年   9篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   73篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   12篇
  2010年   9篇
  2009年   14篇
  2008年   14篇
  2007年   13篇
  2006年   16篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   17篇
  2002年   16篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   7篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   12篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   5篇
  1991年   3篇
  1988年   5篇
  1987年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1983年   4篇
  1981年   3篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   7篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   3篇
  1969年   8篇
  1968年   11篇
  1967年   11篇
  1966年   8篇
  1941年   2篇
排序方式: 共有524条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
23.
Two cross-setional studies examined how infants learn the location of visual events. In Experiment 1, infants of 4, 8, and 12 mo of age learned to turn one way to view a novel pattern. In a subsequent transfer task, they were rotated to face the opposite side of the room. The 4-mo-old infants tended to err by repeating their previously learned response, but within 16-20 trials their performance was comparable to the higher levels maintained by the older infants. These results suggest that young infants learn the location of the pattern primarily in terms of response cues, whereas older infants employ both response cues and place cues. Experiment 2 was designed to independently assess the use of response cues and place cues by infants of 4, 8, 12, and 16 mo of age. All infants were able to rapidly learn and remember the location of the novel pattern when they were given response cues. There was a gradual emergence of place-cue use associated with age. It is suggested that the decrease in infant egocentricity in such spatial localization tasks may in fact reflect an age-related increase in the variety of reliable cues responded to by infants.  相似文献   
24.
25.
26.
The psychological works of Charles Bonnet of Geneva (1720–1793) have customarily been read as part of the mentalistic traditions of associationism or Leibnizianism. They are better understood as part of a campaign—directed against the radical Enlightenment —to reclaim the human body for religion by the paradoxical strategy of incorporating a materialist conception of the human mind into the framework of divine providence. Bonnet's embodied psychology treats the soul as an intrinsically indeterminate force, and attributes the capacity of the mind to organize experience to highly specialized fibers, providentially constructed to correspond to the created world.  相似文献   
27.
Deutsch  Max 《Philosophical Studies》2020,177(12):3935-3957
Philosophical Studies - Advocates of conceptual engineering as a method of philosophy face a dilemma: either they are ignorant of how conceptual engineering can be implemented, or else it is...  相似文献   
28.
Lewis  Max 《Philosophical Studies》2020,177(8):2323-2340
Philosophical Studies - Pessimists think that there is something wrong with relying on deference for one’s moral beliefs—at least if one is morally mature. Call this no deference. They...  相似文献   
29.
Substantial evidence exists that negotiators frequently fail to attain readily available and mutually beneficial outcomes. This paper provides a preliminary model of why these failures occur. We assume that negotiators are decision makers, and that their errors derive from cognitive processes ignored by utility-maximization theories. We focus on one part of the model: the systematic tendency to ignore the cognitions of opponent negotiators. Empirical evidence clarifying negotiators' cognitive processes is generated using verbal protocol techniques in a controlled negotiation task. The results show that subjects simplify the negotiation task, in part by ignoring contingencies introduced by the knowledge possessed by their opponents. The discussion focuses on how subjects simplify the task and how the decision-making perspective helps redirect the negotiation literature.  相似文献   
30.
Murrie DC  Cornell DG 《心理评价》2002,14(4):390-396
How well do brief screening measures correspond with a full-scale assessment of psychopathy among juvenile offenders? This study compared 3 independent screening measures (the Antisocial Process Screening Device [APSD] Self-Report [A. A. Caputo, P. J. Frick, & S. L. Brodsky, 1999], the APSD Staff Rating [P. J. Frick & R. D. Hare, 2001], and the Psychopathy Content Scale [D. C. Murrie & D. G. Cornell, 2000] on the Millon Adolescent Clinical Inventory [T. Millon, 1993]) with the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL:YV; A. E. Forth, D. S. Kosson, & R. D. Hare, in press) in a sample of 117 incarcerated male juveniles. Modest correlations (.30-.49) were found between PCL:YV scores and those of the 3 screening measures, and there was moderate accuracy (67%-82%) in identifying youth who scored relatively high (> or = 25) on the PCL:YV. Although these results support the construct of adolescent psychopathy, they indicate substantial limitations in the use of psychopathy screening measures with juvenile offenders.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号