首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   176篇
  免费   10篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   7篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   10篇
  2016年   11篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   25篇
  2012年   10篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   12篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   6篇
  2007年   9篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   3篇
  2004年   7篇
  2003年   6篇
  2002年   3篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   2篇
  1992年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1985年   1篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   1篇
  1982年   1篇
排序方式: 共有186条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
91.
Inclusivist, exclusivist, and pluralist attitudes toward other religions interact in complex ways within the Mormon faith. Hence, a course on the world’s religions at LDS-sponsored Brigham Young University presents an interesting case study in this context. Through survey data and statistical analysis this article attempts to examine the effect of this course over students’ convictions in their own faith, particularly in relation to the inclusivist vs. exclusivist spectrum. Findings suggest that the sympathetic exposure to other religions in this course did not decrease students’ confessional commitment to Mormonism although it reshaped it in a more inclusivist direction. The religious make-up of students’ families and communities of upbringing is also shown to play a role in this conclusion.  相似文献   
92.
This study assessed whether a boy with profound multiple disabilities and minimal motor behavior would be able to control environmental stimulation using repeated eye blinks with a newly developed microswitch (i.e. an electronically regulated optic sensor mounted on an eyeglasses frame). The study was carried out according to an ABAB design and included a 3 month post‐intervention check. Data showed that the boy had a large increase in the target response (repeated eye blinks) to activate the microswitch and produce environmental stimulation during the B (intervention) phases. This performance was maintained at the post‐intervention check. Practical and developmental implications of the findings were discussed. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
93.
Spatial updating allows people to keep track of the self-to-object relations during movement. Previous studies demonstrated that physical movement enhanced spatial updating in remote environments, but failed to find the same effect in described environments. However, these studies mainly considered rotation as a physical movement, without examining other types of movement, such as walking. We investigated how walking affects spatial updating within described environments. Using the judgement of relative directions task, we compared the effects of imagination of rotation, physical rotation, and walking on spatial updating. Spatial updating was evaluated in terms of accuracy and response times in different perspectives, and by calculating two indexes, namely the encoding and sensorimotor alignment effects. As regards response times, we found that in the imagination of rotation and physical rotation conditions the encoding alignment effect was higher than the sensorimotor alignment effect, while in the walking condition this gap disappeared. We interpreted these results in terms of an enhanced link between allocentric and sensorimotor representations, due to the information acquired through walking.  相似文献   
94.
Recent findings have shown that processing numerical magnitude may interact with finger movements through goal-directed movements. Here we tested these number-finger interactions in a response-effect (R-E) paradigm. During a learning phase, participants read meaningless consonant-vowel (CV) syllables immediately followed by unrelated opening or closing finger movements; during a transfer test, they again named these CV syllables in response to processing a small or a large number. The results showed that responding to a large magnitude number during the transfer phase was slower in an incompatible situation, that is, when the answer was the CV syllable that had been associated to a grip closure during the learning phase. This interference effect demonstrates that ideomotor principles can account for the link between the meaning of numbers and the perception of actions through an anticipated-magnitude code.  相似文献   
95.
According to the active system consolidation theory, memory consolidation during sleep relies on the reactivation of newly encoded memory representations. This reactivation is orchestrated by the interplay of sleep slow oscillations, spindles, and theta, which are in turn modulated by certain neurotransmitters like GABA to enable long-lasting plastic changes in the memory store. Here we asked whether the GABAergic system and associated changes in sleep oscillations are functionally related to memory reactivation during sleep. We administered the GABAA agonist zolpidem (10 mg) in a double-blind placebo-controlled study. To specifically focus on the effects on memory reactivation during sleep, we experimentally induced such reactivations by targeted memory reactivation (TMR) with learning-associated reminder cues presented during post-learning slow-wave sleep (SWS). Zolpidem significantly enhanced memory performance with TMR during sleep compared with placebo. Zolpidem also increased the coupling of fast spindles and theta to slow oscillations, although overall the power of slow spindles and theta was reduced compared with placebo. In an uncorrected exploratory analysis, memory performance was associated with slow spindle responses to TMR in the zolpidem condition, whereas it was associated with fast spindle responses in placebo. These findings provide tentative first evidence that GABAergic activity may be functionally implicated in memory reactivation processes during sleep, possibly via its effects on slow oscillations, spindles and theta as well as their interplay.

Sleep supports the consolidation of newly acquired memories (Mednick et al. 2011; Klinzing et al. 2019). According to the active system consolidation theory, new memories and their associated neuronal activation patterns become spontaneously reactivated (replayed) following learning in the sleeping brain (Wilson and McNaughton 1994; Diekelmann and Born 2010). These reactivations allow for the redistribution and integration of the memory representations from hippocampal to neocortical sites for long-term storage (Rasch and Born 2007; Klinzing et al. 2019). Memory reactivation during sleep has been proposed to rely on the synchronized interplay of electrophysiological oscillations characteristic of non–rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, mainly neocortical slow oscillations (SOs, <1 Hz), thalamocortical spindles (9–15 Hz), and hippocampal ripples (80–200 Hz) (Mölle et al. 2009; Staresina et al. 2015; Helfrich et al. 2018; Ngo et al. 2020). Particularly, sleep spindles and their intricate phase coupling to SO have been suggested to be mechanistically involved in memory consolidation processes during sleep (Ulrich 2016; Antony et al. 2019). It has been proposed that memories become reinstated by spindle events, specifically during the up-state of slow oscillations, allowing for the flow of information between different brain sites as well as the induction of lasting structural and functional plastic changes in the learning-associated neuronal networks (Rosanova and Ulrich 2005; Peyrache and Seibt 2020). In addition to sleep spindles, neocortical and hippocampal theta activity (4–8 Hz) is also phase-locked to SO during NREM sleep (Gonzalez et al. 2018; Cox et al. 2019; Krugliakova et al. 2020), and this coupling has been related to memory consolidation during sleep (Schreiner et al. 2018).A number of neuromodulators seem to be involved in the generation of sleep spindles, SO and associated memory processing, most notably GABA (γ-aminobutyric acid), which is the major inhibitory neurotransmitter (Lancel 1999; Ulrich et al. 2018). Sleep spindles and sleep-dependent memory processing can be boosted by targeting the GABAergic system pharmacologically (Mednick et al. 2013). Zolpidem is one of the most frequently used drugs in this regard, binding to GABAA receptors at the same location as benzodiazepines, thereby acting as a GABAA receptor agonist (Lemmer 2007). Zolpidem increases the time spent in slow-wave sleep (SWS) and reduces the amount of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (Kanno et al. 2000; Uchimura et al. 2006; Zhang et al. 2020). Zolpidem also increases the density and power of sleep spindles (Dijk et al. 2010; Lundahl et al. 2012; Mednick et al. 2013; Niknazar et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2020) as well as the coupling of spindles to SO (Niknazar et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2020), and it was further found to enhance declarative memory consolidation during sleep, with postsleep performance improvements being associated with higher spindle density and spindle power as well as with SO–spindle coupling (Kaestner et al. 2013; Mednick et al. 2013; Zhang et al. 2020).However, it remains unclear whether the changes in sleep stages, sleep spindles, and SO–spindle coupling after pharmacological manipulation with zolpidem are functionally related to the mechanisms underlying sleep-dependent memory consolidation such as memory reactivation. Over the last few years, targeted memory reactivation (TMR) has been increasingly applied to manipulate memory reactivation during sleep experimentally by presenting learning-associated reminder cues like odors or sounds (Oudiette and Paller 2013; Hu et al. 2020; Klinzing and Diekelmann 2020). TMR biases sleep-related neuronal replay events toward the reactivated memory contents (Lewis and Bendor 2019) and enhances subsequent recall performance (Rudoy et al. 2009; Diekelmann et al. 2011; Schreiner et al. 2015; Cairney et al. 2018). Although a few studies observed modulations of SOs (Rihm et al. 2014), sleep spindles (Cox et al. 2014), and SO–spindle coupling (Bar et al. 2020) with TMR during sleep, studies on the role of specific neurotransmitters and particularly on the role of GABAergic neurotransmission and associated changes in sleep oscillations for targeted memory reactivation are entirely lacking. One previous study tested the effect of pharmacologically increased GABAergic activity by administering the benzodiazepine clonazepam after cued reactivation of a declarative memory during wakefulness (Rodríguez et al. 2013). Clonazepam increased memory performance when it was administered after reactivation with an incomplete reminder cue, suggesting that increasing GABAergic neurotransmission may enhance the restabilization of reactivated declarative memories in humans during wakefulness.In the present study, we tested the effect of modulating GABAergic activity with zolpidem on targeted memory reactivation during sleep and associated changes in sleep spindles as well as SO–spindle and SO–theta coupling. We hypothesized that zolpidem enhances the beneficial effects of targeted memory reactivation on memory performance and that this enhancement is associated with increases in spindle density, spindle power, SO–spindle coupling, and possibly SO–theta coupling, and the amount of SWS. Participants were trained on a memory task including 30 sound–word associations in the evening (Forcato et al. 2020) and received an oral dose of 10 mg zolpidem (n = 11) or placebo (n = 11) after training before a full night of sleep in the sleep lab (Fig. 1). During the night, incomplete reminder cues (sounds + first syllable of the associated words) were played again via in-ear headphones during SWS. The next morning, participants were trained on an interference memory task to probe the stability of the original memory, which was tested 30 min later.Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Experimental design and memory task. (A) All subjects took part in a training session at ∼22.30, were administered with placebo (n = 11) or 10 mg of zolpidem (n = 11) before going to bed at 23:00, and received targeted memory reactivation during the first SWS period. After ∼8 h of sleep, in the morning, subjects learned an interference task and were tested on the original memory task in a testing session 30 min after the interference task. (B) Training: First, subjects were presented with 30 sound–word associations for learning. For each association, the sound was presented first for 2900 msec. The sound then continued accompanied by the word written on the screen and spoken aloud for 1500 msec. After a 4000-msec break, the next association was presented in the same way. After all associations were presented once, participants completed an immediate cued recall test. For each association, the sound was presented for 2900 msec. The sound then continued accompanied by the first syllable of the associated word for 1500 msec. Participants were then given 5000 msec to say the complete word aloud (sound continued during the entire period). Independently of their response, the correct answer was then presented on the screen and via headphones for 1500 msec. Reactivation: Each sound was first presented alone for an average of 2900 msec; the sound then continued accompanied by the first syllable of each word for another 1500 msec. After a 7000-msec break, the next sound–syllable pair was presented until all 30 pairs had been presented once. Testing: Each sound was presented for 500 msec and then the sound continued and subjects had 5000 msec to say the associated word aloud. After a break of 4000 msec, the procedure continued for the rest of the 30 associations. Adapted from Forcato et al. (2020).  相似文献   
96.
Neighbourhood perceptions have important implications for individual well‐being, yet there is limited research focused on variables that predict these perceptions. This study proposes that previous engagement in collective action mediates the relationship between neighbourhood bonding social capital and neighbourhood perceptions. Structural equation modelling with a large, nationally representative sample (N = 25,370) is used to test the hypotheses. The findings suggest that bonding social capital has a positive, direct effect on both neighbourhood perceptions and collective action. Collective action has a negative, direct association with neighbourhood perceptions and bonding social capital has a negative, indirect effect on neighbourhood perceptions via collective action. Although the full model explains 28% of the variance in neighbourhood perceptions, bonding social capital only explains 5% of the variance in collective action.  相似文献   
97.
Calamari E  Pini M 《Adolescence》2003,38(150):287-303
The study examined the relationships among dissociative experiences, anger proneness, and attachment styles in late adolescent females. One hundred sixty-two college students (mean age = 17.5 years) were assessed using the Dissociative Experience Scale (DES), the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI), and the Adult Attachment Questionnaire (AAQ), a self-report tool for assessing attachment styles (avoidant, secure, and ambivalent-resistant) in close relationships of youths and adults. Significant correlations were obtained between DES scores (total and factorial) and STAXI scores (State Anger, Trait Anger, Anger/In, and Anger/Expression), confirming in a nonclinical sample the connection between anger proneness and dissociation described in patients with dissociative disorders. Insecure females, particularly ambivalent ones, scored higher on the DES, supporting van der Kolk's hypothesis of an inverse relationship between secure attachment and dissociative tendency. Moreover, insecurely attached females showed more anger proneness, with some differences between ambivalent and avoidant types. Further research should be conducted to examine these relationships in males, as well as to clarify the role of insecure attachment in anger management and the recourse to dissociation in late adolescence as a protective response to trauma and emotional distress.  相似文献   
98.
In 2 experiments participants solved division problems presented in multiplication-based formats (e.g., 8 x _ = 72) more quickly than division problems presented in division-based formats (e.g., 72 / 8 = _). In contrast, participants solved multiplication problems presented in a division-based format (e.g., _ / 8 = 9) slowly and made many errors. In both experiments, the advantage for multiplication-based formats on division problems was found only for large problems (i.e., those with products or dividends greater than 25). These findings provide support for the view that large single-digit division facts are mediated via multiplication-based representations and that multiplication is the primary mode of representation for both division and multiplication facts.  相似文献   
99.
In a recent paper by Casasanto and Pitt (2019), the authors addressed a debate regarding the role of order and magnitude in SNARC and SNARC-like effects. Their position is that all these effects can be explained by order, while magnitude could only account for a subset of evidence. Although we agree that order can probably explain the majority of these effects, in this commentary we argue that magnitude is still relevant, since there is evidence that cannot be explained based on ordinality alone. We argue that SNARC-like effects can occur for magnitudes not clearly characterized by overlearned ordinality and that magnitude can prevail on order, when the two are pitted against each other. Finally, we propose that different interpretations of the role of order and magnitude depend on the interaction of stimulus properties and task demands.  相似文献   
100.
We demonstrate that a person's eye gaze and his/her competitiveness are closely intertwined in social decision making. In an exploratory examination of this relationship, Study 1 uses field data from a high‐stakes TV game show to demonstrate that the frequency by which contestants gaze at their opponent's eyes predicts their defection in a variant on the prisoner's dilemma. Studies 2 and 3 use experiments to examine the underlying causality and demonstrate that the relationship between gazing and competitive behavior is bi‐directional. In Study 2, fixation on the eyes, compared to the face, increases competitive behavior toward the target in an ultimatum game. In Study 3, we manipulate the framing of a negotiation (cooperative vs. competitive) and use an eye tracker to measure fixation number and time spent fixating on the counterpart's eyes. We find that a competitive negotiation elicits more gazing, which in turn leads to more competitive behavior.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号