全文获取类型
收费全文 | 999篇 |
免费 | 20篇 |
专业分类
1019篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 31篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 42篇 |
2017年 | 48篇 |
2016年 | 45篇 |
2015年 | 25篇 |
2014年 | 44篇 |
2013年 | 107篇 |
2012年 | 51篇 |
2011年 | 52篇 |
2010年 | 36篇 |
2009年 | 44篇 |
2008年 | 50篇 |
2007年 | 39篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 28篇 |
2003年 | 35篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
1921年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1019条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
We examined categorical effects in color appearance in two tasks, which in part differed in the extent to which color naming was explicitly required for the response. In one, we measured the effects of color differences on perceptual grouping for hues that spanned the blue-green boundary, to test whether chromatic differences across the boundary were perceptually exaggerated. This task did not require overt judgments of the perceived colors, and the tendency to group showed only a weak and inconsistent categorical bias. In a second case, we analyzed results from two prior studies of hue scaling of chromatic stimuli (De Valois, De Valois, Switkes, & Mahon, 1997; Malkoc, Kay, & Webster, 2005), to test whether color appearance changed more rapidly around the blue-green boundary. In this task observers directly judge the perceived color of the stimuli and these judgments tended to show much stronger categorical effects. The differences between these tasks could arise either because different signals mediate color grouping and color appearance, or because linguistic categories might differentially intrude on the response to color and/or on the perception of color. Our results suggest that the interaction between language and color processing may be highly dependent on the specific task and cognitive demands and strategies of the observer, and also highlight pronounced individual differences in the tendency to exhibit categorical responses. 相似文献
12.
Dipl.-Psych. Matthias Richter 《Forum der Psychoanalyse》2012,28(1):27-49
The somewhat crude question of whether we need neuropsychotherapy means more precisely whether its neuroscientific perspective can be useful for the psychotherapeutic practice. To answer this question the traditional scientific approaches do not completely suffice. The ??empirical-technical approach?? of quantitative psychotherapy research is of no service here as neuropsychotherapy is only its continuation. Within the framework of this methodology it is not possible to critically reflect on the relevance of neuroscientific research. Such a reflection is, however, the prerequisite for an evaluation of the benefits of neuropsychotherapy. The question about the meaning of neurobiological processes thus leads to the field of epistemology. This ??theoretical approach?? is nonetheless only helpful within limits. It provides an important background of reflection, however it needs to remain abstract compared to psychotherapeutic practice because it does not integrate the specific goals of such practice. What is presented here instead is an ??approach of pragmatic reflection??. This reflection departs from a general purpose of psychotherapy from which the value of neuropsychotherapy can be appropriately estimated. After this clarification the approach of pragmatic reflection can be preliminarily demonstrated with concrete applications of neuroscience in psychotherapy. What is shown hereby is that neuropsychotherapy represents a very specific practice of argumention and action, which implements a logic into psychotherapy that does not fit its contents and goals. 相似文献
13.
14.
Philosophical Studies - This paper focuses on the role of culpability in determining the degree of liability to defensive harm, and asks whether there are any restrictions on when culpability is... 相似文献
15.
Sex Roles - Research has shown that language can be gender-biased; however, little research has investigated the prevalence of this bias in everyday speech. Using recordings sampled from... 相似文献
16.
A general class of labeled sequent calculi is investigated, and necessary and sufficient conditions are given for when such a calculus is sound and complete for a finite-valued logic if the labels are interpreted as sets of truth values (sets-as-signs). Furthermore, it is shown that any finite-valued logic can be given an axiomatization by such a labeled calculus using arbitrary "systems of signs," i.e., of sets of truth values, as labels. The number of labels needed is logarithmic in the number of truth values, and it is shown that this bound is tight. 相似文献
17.
18.
We construct a faithful interpretation of ukasiewicz's logic in product logic (both propositional and predicate). Using known facts it follows that the product predicate logic is not recursively axiomatizable.We prove a completeness theorem for product logic extended by a unary connective of Baaz [1]. We show that Gödel's logic is a sublogic of this extended product logic.We also prove NP-completeness of the set of propositional formulas satisfiable in product logic (resp. in Gödel's logic). 相似文献
19.
Word frequency is one of the strongest determiners of reaction time (RT) in word recognition tasks; it is an important theoretical and methodological variable. The Ku?era and Francis (1967) word frequency count (derived from the 1-million-word Brown corpus) is used by most investigators concerned with the issue of word frequency. Word frequency estimates from the Brown corpus were compared with those from a 131-million-word corpus (the HAL corpus; conversational text gathered from Usenet) in a standard word naming task with 32 subjects. RT was predicted equally well by both corpora for high-frequency words, but the larger corpus provided better predictors for low- and medium-frequency words. Furthermore, the larger corpus provides estimates for 97,261 lexical items; the smaller corpus, for 50,406 items. 相似文献
20.
Matthias Schirn 《Erkenntnis》1990,32(1):27-60
A previous version of this paper was read at the universities of Glasgow, Munich, Oxford and St. Andrews; the penultimate version was presented at the universities of Bologna, Erlangen-Nürnberg, Minnesota, Puerto Rico and at University College, London. I am grateful to Verena Mayer, Eva Picardi, Peter Clark, Michael Dummett, Bill Hart, Andreas Kemmerling, Brian McGuinness, Guillermo E. Rosado Haddock, Joachim Schulte, Leslie Stevenson and especially to Christian Thiel and Crispin Wright for helpful comments. 相似文献