全文获取类型
收费全文 | 304篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
308篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 18篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 31篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 23篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有308条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
152.
Matthew H. Wilder Matt Jones Alaa A. Ahmed Tim Curran Michael C. Mozer 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2013,20(6):1221-1231
As we perform daily activities—driving to work, unlocking the office door, or grabbing a coffee cup—our actions seem automatic and preprogrammed. Nonetheless, routine, well-practiced behavior is continually modulated by incidental experience: In repetitive experimental tasks, recent (~4) trials reliably influence performance and action choice. Psychological theories downplay the significance of sequential effects, explaining them as rapidly decaying perturbations of behavior, with no long-term consequences. We challenged this traditional perspective in two experiments designed to probe the impact of more distant experience, finding evidence for effects spanning up to a thousand intermediate trials. We present a normative theory in which these persistent effects reflect optimal adaptation to a dynamic environment exhibiting varying rates of change. The theory predicts a heavy-tailed decaying influence of past experience, consistent with our data, and suggests that individual incidental experiences are catalogued in a temporally extended memory utilized in order to optimize subsequent behavior. 相似文献
153.
Madeleine A. Fugère Carlos Escoto Alita J. Cousins Matt L. Riggs Paul Haerich 《Sexuality & culture》2008,12(3):169-182
We conducted a review of literature regarding sexual attitudes and double standards, focusing on participant gender and ethnic
background. We found that men had more permissive sexual attitudes than women, and that African Americans had the most permissive
sexual attitudes, followed by White Americans, then by Hispanic Americans and Asian Americans. The literature regarding sexual
double standards was mixed; some studies showed evidence of continued sexual double standards and some studies showed the
absence of sexual double standards. In some studies, men were more likely to endorse the sexual double standard than women.
We found only one article addressing sexual double standards using ethnic background as a quasi-independent variable; this
research revealed that non-North American (Russian and Japanese) samples were more likely to endorse the traditional double
standard, that sex is more acceptable for men than for women.
相似文献
Madeleine A. FugèreEmail: |
154.
Actions in a repeated game can in principle depend on all previous outcomes. Given this vast policy space, human players may often be forced to use heuristics that base actions on incomplete information, such as the outcomes of only the most recent trials. Here it is proven that such bounded rationality is often fully rational, in that the optimal policy based on some limited information about the game's history will be universally optimal (i.e., within the full policy space), provided that one's opponents are restricted to using this same information. It is then shown how this result allows explicit calculation of subgame-perfect equilibria (SPEs) for any repeated or stochastic game. The technique is applied to the iterated Prisoner's Dilemma for the case of 1-back memory. Two classes of SPEs are derived, which exhibit varying degrees of (individually rational) cooperation as a result of repeated interaction. 相似文献
155.
Tim Riesen John McDonnell Jesse W. Johnson Shamby Polychronis Matt Jameson 《Journal of Behavioral Education》2003,12(4):241-259
An adapted alternating treatment design was used to measure the effectiveness of constant time delay and simultaneous prompting procedures within an embedded instruction format on the acquisition of academic skills. Four middle school students with moderate to severe disabilities and the paraprofessionals who supported these students in their general education classes participated in the study. The results of the study indicated that both procedures were effective in promoting the acquisition of the target skills. However, the constant time delay procedure was more effective for two of the students and the simultaneous prompting procedure was more effective for the remaining students. Results are discussed in terms of future research and implications for supporting the inclusion of students with moderate and severe disabilities in general education classes. 相似文献
156.
Matt Stichter 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2011,14(1):73-86
According to Rosalind Hursthouse’s virtue based account of right action, an act is right if it is what a fully virtuous person
would do in that situation. Robert Johnson has criticized the account on the grounds that the actions a non-virtuous person
should take are often uncharacteristic of the virtuous person, and thus Hursthouse’s account of right action is too narrow.
The non-virtuous need to take steps to improve themselves morally, and the fully virtuous person need not take these steps.
So Johnson argues that any virtue based account of right action will have to find a way to ground a moral obligation to improve
oneself. This paper argues that there is an account of virtue that can offer a partial solution to Johnson’s challenge, an
account where virtue is a type of practical skill and in which the virtuous person is seen as having expertise. The paper
references the account of skill acquisition developed by Hubert and Stuart Dreyfus. Their research demonstrates that novices
in a skill have to employ different strategies to act well than the strategies used by the experts, and so the ‘virtue as
skill’ thesis provides support for Johnson’s claim that the actions of the non-virtuous will differ from the virtuous. On
the other hand, their research suggests that there is no separating the commitment to improve yourself from the possession
of expertise, and so the ‘virtue as skill’ thesis has the resources for grounding the obligation to improve oneself in an
account of virtue. 相似文献
157.
Dietary restraint is a prospective risk factor for the development of binge eating and bulimia nervosa. Although many women engage in dietary restraint, relatively few develop binge eating. Dietary restraint may increase susceptibility for binge eating only in individuals who are at genetic risk. Specifically, dietary restraint may be a behavioral exposure factor that activates genetic predispositions for binge eating. We investigated this possibility in 1,678 young adolescent and adult same-sex female twins from the Minnesota Twin Family Study and the Michigan State University Twin Registry. Twin moderation models were used to examine whether levels of dietary restraint moderate genetic and environmental influences on binge eating. Results indicated that genetic and nonshared environmental factors for binge eating increased at higher levels of dietary restraint. These effects were present after controlling for age, body mass index, and genetic and environmental overlap among dietary restraint and binge eating. Results suggest that dietary restraint may be most important for individuals at genetic risk for binge eating and that the combination of these factors could enhance individual differences in risk for binge eating. 相似文献
158.
The consistent association between adolescent sexual initiation (ASI) and risky adult sexual behavior (RASB) has generally been assumed to indicate that ASI has a causal effect on RASB; consequently, it is assumed that delaying ASI will reduce RASB. Yet the ASI-RASB association might be better accounted for by some third variable. We evaluated the causal role of ASI (initiation of oral, anal, or vaginal sex at or before age 16) in influencing RASB in a longitudinal sample of 2,173 twins (followed from ages 11 to 24 or from ages 17 to 29) using two methods: the discordant-twin design and the propensity-score design. The former controlled for unmeasured genetic and shared environmental factors, and the latter controlled for measured nonshared environmental factors. We replicated the link between ASI and RASB reported in previous research, but results from the discordant-twin and propensity-score analyses suggested that this association is better explained by common genetic or environmental risk factors than as a causal effect. These findings suggest that preventing ASI is unlikely to reduce RASB. 相似文献
159.
Klahr AM Rueter MA McGue M Iacono WG Burt SA 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2011,39(5):683-694
Prior studies have indicated that the relationship between parent-child conflict and adolescent antisocial behavior is at
least partially shared environmental in origin. However, all available research on this topic (to our knowledge) relies exclusively
on parent and/or adolescent informant-reports, both of which are subject to various forms of rater bias. As the presence of
significant shared environmental effects has often been attributed to rater bias in the past (Baker et al. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 16:219–235, 2007; Bartels et al. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 42:1351–1359, 2003, Twin Research 7:162–175, 2004; Hewitt et al. Behavior Genetics 22:293–317, 1992), it would be important to confirm that findings of shared environmental mediation persist when even examining (presumably
more objective) observer-ratings of these constructs. The current study thus examined the origins of the relationship between
parent-child conflict and adolescent acting-out behavior, as measured using both observer-ratings and various informant-reports.
Participants included 1,199 adopted and non-adopted adolescents in 610 families from the Sibling Interaction and Behavior
Study (SIBS). Results indicated that parent-child conflict consistently predicts acting-out behavior in adopted adolescents,
and moreover, that this association is equivalent to that in biologically-related adolescents. Most importantly, these findings
did not vary across parent- and adolescent-reported or observer-ratings of parent-child conflict and acting-out behavior.
Such findings argue strongly against rater bias as a primary explanation of shared environmental mediation of the association
between parent-child conflict and adolescent antisocial behavior. 相似文献
160.
G E Matt M F Hovell J M Zakarian J T Bernert J L Pirkle S K Hammond 《Health psychology》2000,19(3):232-241
The reliability and validity of mother's reports of their infants' exposure to secondhand smoke (SHS) were examined in an ethnically diverse sample of low-income, low-education families (N = 141 mothers). At baseline and posttest, smoking mothers reported about their infants' SHS exposure at different locations and by different sources during the previous week. Findings show that mothers can give reliable accounts of the degree to which they contribute to their babies' SHS exposure. Mothers are able to differentiate between their own smoking behavior and the extent to which they expose their infants. Consistent with the overall exposure pattern, exposure caused by the mother and exposure occurring at home showed the strongest associations with biological and environmental measures. These findings suggest that smoking mothers can provide reliable and valid reports of the degree to which their infants are exposed to SHS. 相似文献