全文获取类型
收费全文 | 393篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
404篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 6篇 |
2017年 | 4篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 49篇 |
2012年 | 9篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 22篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 12篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1955年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有404条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
221.
Srivastava S McGonigal KM Richards JM Butler EA Gross JJ 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2006,91(1):143-153
Does expecting positive outcomes--especially in important life domains such as relationships--make these positive outcomes more likely? In a longitudinal study of dating couples, the authors tested whether optimists (who have a cognitive disposition to expect positive outcomes) and their romantic partners are more satisfied in their relationships, and if so, whether this is due to optimists perceiving greater support from their partners. In cross-sectional analyses, both optimists and their partners indicated greater relationship satisfaction, an effect that was mediated by optimists' greater perceived support. When the couples engaged in a conflict conversation, optimists and their partners saw each other as engaging more constructively during the conflict, which in turn led both partners to feel that the conflict was better resolved 1 week later. In a 1-year follow-up, men's optimism predicted relationship status. Effects of optimism were mediated by the optimists' perceived support, which appears to promote a variety of beneficial processes in romantic relationships. 相似文献
222.
Murray Singer Nathalie Gagnon Eric Richards 《Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale》2002,56(1):41-57
This study scrutinized people's ability to apply different strategies to randomly intermixed immediate and delayed test items. In three experiments, participants first read one set of stories. Later, they read more stories, and after each one, answered intermixed questions about that story and one of the earlier ones. The experiments cumulatively manipulated amount of delay, test probe plausibility, probe relation (explicit, paraphrase, inference), and testing procedure (mixed versus uniform delay). Using signal detection response criterion as the index of strategy, we contrasted the single criterion hypothesis, according to which one text retrieval criterion is applied to all test items, and a multiple-criterion hypothesis. The results consistently favoured the multiple-criterion hypothesis. The results also indicated that the presence of immediate and delayed probes mutually influence one another: Less extreme signal detection criteria were adopted under mixed than uniform testing. It was concluded that text retrieval strategy is continually calibrated with reference to the quality of the test probes. 相似文献
223.
Lejuez CW Read JP Kahler CW Richards JB Ramsey SE Stuart GL Strong DR Brown RA 《Journal of experimental psychology. Applied》2002,8(2):75-84
The present study (N = 86) sought to evaluate a laboratory-based behavioral measure of risk taking (the Balloon Analogue Risk Task; BART) and to test associations between this measure and self-report measures of risk-related constructs as well as self-reported real-world risk behaviors. The BART evidenced sound experimental properties, and riskiness on the BART was correlated with scores on measures of sensation seeking, impulsivity, and deficiencies in behavioral constraint. Also, riskiness on the BART was correlated with the self-reported occurrence of addictive, health, and safety risk behaviors, with the task accounting for variance in these behaviors beyond that accounted for by demographics and self-report measures of risk-related constructs. These results indicate that the BART may be a useful tool in the assessment of risk taking. 相似文献
224.
Whitman RichardsBrendan D. McKay Diana Richards 《Journal of mathematical psychology》2002,46(3):338-351
Complex decision making typically involves many agents attempting to aggregate many alternatives. If agents' preferences are unconstrained, cyclic outcomes are highly probable. In contrast, we show that if agents share similar models of the choice domain, then a stable collective outcome occurs with about 90% probability. These results have implications for systems of interacting agents, such as group decisions by individuals in social settings or interpretations of sense data by competing perceptual modules of a brain. 相似文献
225.
226.
Jerry M. Lewis J. M. Rabbie Thomas Berg Michael Argyle Karin Aronsson Roger Ingham Neil Frude A. M. Munton Duncan Cramer John Shotter Ray Holland Nobleza Asuncion-Lande Ray Crozier Candida Peterson Chris Spencer Victor J. Callan Jack Martin N. L. Reinsch Anthony Ryle David Canter Jill Robson H. R. Beech Duncan Moss David Fontana Tom Richards 《Current Psychology》1989,8(1):49-87
227.
228.
Richards JB Zhang L Mitchell SH de Wit H 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1999,71(2):121-143
Little is known about the acute effects of drugs of abuse on impulsivity and self-control. In this study, impulsivity was assessed in humans using a computer task that measured delay and probability discounting. Discounting describes how much the value of a reward (or punisher) is decreased when its occurrence is either delayed or uncertain. Twenty-four healthy adult volunteers ingested a moderate dose of ethanol (0.5 or 0.8 g/kg ethanol: n = 12 at each dose) or placebo before completing the discounting task. In the task the participants were given a series of choices between a small, immediate, certain amount of money and $10 that was either delayed (0, 2, 30, 180, or 365 days) or probabilistic (i.e., certainty of receipt was 1.0, .9, .75, .5, or .25). The point at which each individual was indifferent between the smaller immediate or certain reward and the $10 delayed or probabilistic reward was identified using an adjusting-amount procedure. The results indicated that (a) delay and probability discounting were well described by a hyperbolic function; (b) delay and probability discounting were positively correlated within subjects; (c) delay and probability discounting were moderately correlated with personality measures of impulsivity; and (d) alcohol had no effect on discounting. 相似文献
229.
Leslie L. Davidson Susan J. Hughes Marcus Richards 《Personality and individual differences》1987,8(6):961-963
Measures of injury were obtained in a birth cohort of 953 children in Wales along with maternal Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI) and children's Behaviour Screening Questionnaire (BSQ) data. While there was no relationship between maternal E and child injury, children of low-N mothers had significantly fewer injuries than those of medium of high-N mothers. The magnitude of this trend remained after controlling for the BSQ variables also relating to injury, but was no longer significant in the case of management disorder or of sex. Moreover, while the association was observed for families with only one or two children, this was not the case for families with three or more children. 相似文献
230.