全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3536篇 |
免费 | 45篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3583篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 31篇 |
2020年 | 49篇 |
2019年 | 71篇 |
2018年 | 103篇 |
2017年 | 90篇 |
2016年 | 96篇 |
2015年 | 83篇 |
2014年 | 86篇 |
2013年 | 409篇 |
2012年 | 135篇 |
2011年 | 127篇 |
2010年 | 101篇 |
2009年 | 75篇 |
2008年 | 117篇 |
2007年 | 118篇 |
2006年 | 107篇 |
2005年 | 118篇 |
2004年 | 116篇 |
2003年 | 108篇 |
2002年 | 110篇 |
2001年 | 54篇 |
2000年 | 48篇 |
1999年 | 58篇 |
1998年 | 62篇 |
1997年 | 38篇 |
1996年 | 66篇 |
1995年 | 55篇 |
1994年 | 57篇 |
1993年 | 53篇 |
1992年 | 54篇 |
1991年 | 40篇 |
1990年 | 44篇 |
1989年 | 44篇 |
1988年 | 35篇 |
1987年 | 40篇 |
1986年 | 37篇 |
1985年 | 54篇 |
1984年 | 35篇 |
1983年 | 50篇 |
1982年 | 42篇 |
1981年 | 35篇 |
1980年 | 36篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 31篇 |
1977年 | 28篇 |
1976年 | 26篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 21篇 |
排序方式: 共有3583条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
Milburn NG Ayala G Rice E Batterham P Rotheram-Borus MJ 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2006,12(4):658-672
This article examines how newly homeless adolescents' discrimination experiences were associated with exiting homelessness after 6 months. A sample of 262 homeless adolescents, aged 12 to 20 years, were recruited and followed longitudinally (6-month retention rate = 88%). Discrimination was related to being gay, lesbian, or bisexual (LGB). Discrimination from family was related to exiting homelessness. Other than those who were LGB, adolescents who reported discrimination from their families were more likely to exit homelessness than adolescents who did not report such discrimination. Suggestions for future research include focusing on the experiences of LGB homeless adolescents, the role of families in the lives of homeless adolescents, and other aspects of discrimination, including salience, frequency, intensity, and duration. 相似文献
942.
McAdams DP Anyidoho NA Brown C Huang YT Kaplan B Machado MA 《Journal of personality》2004,72(4):761-784
Dispositional traits and life narratives represent two different levels of personality that have not previously been empirically linked. The current study tested five hypotheses connecting Big-Five traits to life-narrative indices of emotional tone, theme, and structure. Students (Study 1) and adults (Study 2) completed a self-report measure of the Big-Five traits and provided extended written accounts of either ten (students) or eight (adults) key life-narrative scenes, including life high points, low points, and turning points. Content analysis of the narrative data revealed that for both samples Neuroticism was positively associated with an emotionally negative life-narrative tone, Agreeableness was correlated with narrative themes of communion (e.g., friendship, caring for others), and Openness was strongly associated with the structural complexity of life narrative accounts. Contrary to prediction, however, Conscientiousness was not consistently associated with themes of agency (e.g., achievement, self-mastery) and Extraversion was unrelated to positive narrative tone. The results are discussed in the context of contemporary research and theorizing on the narrative study of lives and the relation of narrative research in personality to more conventional, trait-based approaches. 相似文献
943.
Bornstein RF Geiselman KJ Gallagher HA Ng HM Hughes EE Languirand MA 《Journal of personality assessment》2004,82(1):104-113
Research supports the construct validity of the Relationship Profile Test (RPT; Bornstein & Languirand, 2003), a 30-item, self-report measure of destructive overdependence (DO), dysfunctional detachment (DD), and healthy dependency. In this investigation, we assessed the relationships of gender, gender role, and gender role stereotype ratings to RPT subscale scores. In Study 1, we replicated earlier patterns of gender differences in RPT scores, assessed cross-sample consistency in gender difference effect sizes, and provided preliminary nonclinical norms for the RPT subscales. Study 2 showed that--as expected--DO items are perceived as stereotypically feminine, whereas DD items are perceived as stereotypically masculine. In Study 3, we examined the relationships of RPT subscale scores to masculinity, femininity, and androgyny scores. We discuss the theoretical, empirical, and clinical implications of these findings. 相似文献
944.
To assess the comfort of athletic trainers in providing care to athletes of the same- and opposite-sex, the Gender Comfort in Athletic Training Questionnaire was developed. Cronbach alpha was .72. The 8-item questionnaire was mailed to 235 participants, who were senior athletic training students and clinical instructors affiliated with 14 university athletic training programs. 192 questionnaires were returned (82%). Only one t test showed a significant mean difference in reported comfort between women (n= 103, M=2.8, SD= 0.8; disagree) and men (n= 89, M=3.1, SD=0.8; neutral) on the statement: I feel more comfortable providing athletic training services to male athletes than female athletes. The women and men in this sample were similar in their self-ratings of comfort in providing athletic training services to male and female athletes. 相似文献
945.
Categorizing facial identities, emotions, and genders: attention to high- and low-spatial frequencies by children and adults 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Three age groups of participants (5-6 years, 7-8 years, adults) matched faces on the basis of facial identity. The procedure involved either low- or high-pass filtered faces or hybrid faces composed from two faces associated with different spatial bandwidths. The comparison stimuli were unfiltered faces. In the three age groups, the data indicated a significant bias for processing of low-pass information in priority. In a second task, participants were asked to identify the emotion (smiling or grimacing) or gender (male or female) of hybrid high-pass/low-pass faces. Opposite results emerged in the two tasks irrespective of the age group; the gender discrimination task indicated a bias for low-pass information, and the emotion task indicated a bias for high-pass information. These differences suggest independent processing routes for functionally different types of information such as emotion, gender, and identity. These routes are already established by 5 years of age. 相似文献
946.
The present study explored how European American women in midlife transition think about their changing bodies and related
sociocultural discourses. The work was informed by theory that illuminates a process in which middle-aged individuals critically
evaluate the meaning structures used to make sense of the world and self and transform these schemes for personal relevance.
In-depth interviews were conducted with 20 women transitioning into their middle-aged years. Data were analyzed using constant
comparison processes. Analyses revealed two major ideological adjustments in participants' thinking, including (a) the shift
from an emphasis upon external to internal aspects of the self and (b) the questioning of dominant social discourses about
the female body. Although these ideological shifts seemingly contributed to participants' acceptance of their bodies at midlife,
participants also expressed ambivalence about their appearances and related social discourses. 相似文献
947.
Activation of adenosine receptors in the posterior cingulate cortex impairs memory retrieval in the rat 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Pereira GS Rossato JI Sarkis JJ Cammarota M Bonan CD Izquierdo I 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2005,83(3):217-223
Adenosine A1 and A2A receptor agonists and antagonists have been reported to alter learning and memory. The aim of our study was to investigate the involvement of adenosinergic system in memory retrieval into posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) of Wistar rats. To clarify this question, we tested specifics agonist and antagonists of adenosine A1 and A2A receptors in rats submitted to a one-trial inhibitory avoidance task. The stimulation of adenosine A1 and A2A receptors by CPA and CGS21680, respectively, impaired memory retrieval for inhibitory avoidance task, into PCC. These findings provide behavioral evidence for the role of adenosinergic system in the memory retrieval into PCC. 相似文献
948.
This observational study describes the early development of attention and discractibility. Under several conditions of distraction, 172 children at 10, 26, and 42 months of age played with toys. Attention to the toys was coded as casual, settled, or focused. All 3 levels of attention changed with age, withcasual attention decreasing and focused attention increasing. The 10-month-olds were more distractible than the other children, even during focused attention. The infants were most distracted by the auditory-visual distractor, whereas the oldest children were most distracted by the visual distractor. Some 42-month-olds showed evidence of being more focused in the presence of distractors. Overall, the results point to a developmental transition in the processes underlying attention during play. 相似文献
949.
Popp Danielle Donovan Roxanne A. Crawford Mary Marsh Kerry L. Peele Melanie 《Sex roles》2003,48(7-8):317-325
Considerable research has shown that people have stereotypical beliefs about the speech and communication style of women and men. There is less research about stereotypes of Black people's speech, and none that jointly or comparably investigates communication stereotypes as a function of both gender and race. In this study, White college students (n = 111) rated a fictional character's speech on 36 pairs of words characteristic of communication style (e.g., emotional–unemotional) and also generated dialogue for the character. Targets' race and sex were varied. Results showed that beliefs about speech style were stronger for race than gender. Black speakers, both women and men, were rated as more direct and emotional, and less socially appropriate and playful, than White speakers. The dialogue generated by participants for Black speakers was less grammatical and more profane than for White speakers. Gender effects were consistent with earlier research but suggest a weakening of stereotypes; women's speech was seen as somewhat less direct and more emotional than men's speech. Beliefs about speech and communication style are important because they may function not only to describe what is but to prescribe what should be in social interaction. 相似文献
950.
Wagstaff GF MacVeigh J Boston R Scott L Brunas-Wagstaff J Cole J 《The Journal of psychology》2003,137(1):17-28