全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2930篇 |
免费 | 86篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3018篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 14篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 36篇 |
2019年 | 55篇 |
2018年 | 73篇 |
2017年 | 61篇 |
2016年 | 67篇 |
2015年 | 55篇 |
2014年 | 61篇 |
2013年 | 360篇 |
2012年 | 105篇 |
2011年 | 96篇 |
2010年 | 79篇 |
2009年 | 69篇 |
2008年 | 91篇 |
2007年 | 107篇 |
2006年 | 96篇 |
2005年 | 101篇 |
2004年 | 107篇 |
2003年 | 89篇 |
2002年 | 95篇 |
2001年 | 47篇 |
2000年 | 41篇 |
1999年 | 57篇 |
1998年 | 54篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 61篇 |
1995年 | 53篇 |
1994年 | 53篇 |
1993年 | 48篇 |
1992年 | 55篇 |
1991年 | 35篇 |
1990年 | 39篇 |
1989年 | 41篇 |
1988年 | 33篇 |
1987年 | 39篇 |
1986年 | 34篇 |
1985年 | 49篇 |
1984年 | 32篇 |
1983年 | 45篇 |
1982年 | 40篇 |
1981年 | 31篇 |
1980年 | 32篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 25篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1976年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
1974年 | 23篇 |
1973年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有3018条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Autobiographical memory research documents increased access in the number of memories recalled by emerging adults (ages 18–25)
with stable, clearly defined self-concepts. Very little research has examined the relationship between self-concept clarity
and the episodic and semantic components of emerging adults’ autobiographical recollections. This study examined the relationship
between episodic and semantic autobiographical memory and self-concept clarity in 100 undergraduate students. Contrary to
previous findings, which indicated that stable self-beliefs may be used to facilitate access to episodic memories, the results
suggested that self-concept clarity may not be associated with memory specificity. Instead, self-concept clarity was associated
with recall of events involving cooperative social interactions. Results further showed that older age was associated with
higher SCC. The implications of these results are discussed within the context of the theoretical literature concerning the
relation between autobiographical memory and the development of the self-concept during emerging adulthood. 相似文献
102.
Teri?Brister Mary?A.?CavaleriEmail author S.?Serene?Olin Sa?Shen Barbara?J.?Burns Kimberly?E.?Hoagwood 《Journal of child and family studies》2012,21(3):439-442
This brief report describes results from an evaluation of NAMI Basics, a peer-delivered family education program for family
caregivers of children and adolescents with mental illness. Over six classes, family members are given information (e.g. education
about mental illness and treatments), skills training (e.g. family communication skills) and advocacy support. We report data
from 36 caregivers who completed pre and posttest instruments measuring self-care, empowerment, and family problem-solving
and communication skills. Results showed significant improvements in self-care, empowerment, and “incendiary” family communication
after participation in the program. Results suggest that NAMI Basics may improve both parental functioning and familial processes.
Implications and future directions are discussed. 相似文献
103.
The commitment of transplant physicians to protect the physical and psychological health of potential donors is fundamental to the process of living donor organ transplantation. It is appropriate that strict regulations to govern an individual's decision to donate have been developed. Some may argue that adherence to such regulations creates a doctor-patient relationship that is rooted in paternalism, which is in drastic contrast with a doctor-patient relationship that is rooted in patients' autonomy, characteristic of most other operative interventions. In this article we analyze the similarities between cosmetic plastic surgery and living donor surgery as examples of surgeries governed by different ethical principles. It is interesting that, while the prevailing ethical approach in living donor surgery is based on paternalism, the ethical principle guiding cosmetic surgery is respect for patients' autonomy. The purpose of this article is not to criticize either practice, but to suggest that, given the similarities between the two procedures, both operative interventions should be guided by the same ethical principle: a respect for patients' autonomy. We further suggest that if living organ donation valued donors' autonomy as much as cosmetic plastic surgery does, we might witness a wider acceptance of and increase in living organ donation. 相似文献
104.
Objective: To empirically identify the appropriate symptom threshold for hyperactivity-impulsivity for diagnosis of ADHD in adults. Method: Participants were 88 adults (M [SD] age = 41.69 [11.78] years, 66% female, 16% minority) meeting formal DSM-IV criteria for ADHD combined or predominantly inattentive subtypes based on a structured diagnostic interview keyed to DSM-IV (Conners' Adult ADHD Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV [CAADID]). All participants also completed the Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS), which was normed on the general adult population and includes subscales for DSM-IV inattentive and DSM-IV hyperactive-impulsive symptoms. A T-score threshold of 65 (at least 1.5 SD above population mean) on the CAARS DSM-IV hyperactive-impulsive dimension was used to identify participants with empirically elevated symptom severity. Results: Of 88 participating adults, 48 (55%) had a T-score of at least 65 (1.5 SD) on the CAARS DSM-IV Hyperactive-Impulsive scale. Of these, only 25 (52%) met the DSM-IV cutoff of six hyperactive-impulsive symptoms on the CAADID. Thus, approximately half of those who reported empirically elevated hyperactive-impulsive complaints on the CAARS did not concurrently meet the six-symptom DSM-IV cutoff on the CAADID. An alternative cutoff of four hyperactive-impulsive symptoms on the CAADID captured 39 (81%) cases identified by the CAARS. Conclusion: In adults, mandating at least six hyperactive-impulsive symptoms excludes a significant percentage (almost half) of adults who are at least 1.5 SD above the population mean on a dimensional measure of hyperactivity-impulsivity. These data provide a compelling basis for lowering the symptom threshold of hyperactivity-impulsivity for adults in the DSM-5. 相似文献
105.
Mary T. Hawkins Kristi-Ann Villagonzalo Ann V. Sanson John W. Toumbourou Primrose Letcher Craig A. Olsson 《Journal of Adult Development》2012,19(2):88-99
Little is known about positive development across the transition from adolescence to young adulthood. The purpose of this paper was to use longitudinal data from a large community sample, the Australian Temperament Project, to investigate prospective associations between positive development at age 19?C20?years and changes in social, health, and behavioral outcomes at 23?C24?years. Data from 890 young adults (61.7% women) were analyzed using path analysis. Emotional health, physical well-being, friendship quality, reduced antisocial behavior, and positive development in young adulthood were predicted by higher levels of positive development in late adolescence, after controlling for baseline levels of respective outcomes, gender, and family socioeconomic status. Parent relationship quality and alcohol use were not significantly predicted. Based on these findings, interventions promoting positive development during adolescence should be investigated as a potential means of yielding positive social, health, and behavioral consequences well into young adulthood. 相似文献
106.
Although the study of brain states is an old one in neuroscience, there has been growing interest in brain state specification owing to MRI studies tracing brain connectivity at rest. In this review, we summarize recent research on three relatively well-described brain states: the resting, alert, and meditation states. We explore the neural correlates of maintaining a state or switching between states, and argue that the anterior cingulate cortex and striatum play a critical role in state maintenance, whereas the insula has a major role in switching between states. Brain state may serve as a predictor of performance in a variety of perceptual, memory, and problem solving tasks. Thus, understanding brain states is critical for understanding human performance. 相似文献
107.
When searching for an object, an observer holds a representation of the target in mind while scanning the scene. If the observer repeats the search, performance may become more efficient as the observer hones this target representation, or "search template," to match the specific demands of the search task. An effective search template must have two characteristics: It must reliably discriminate the target from the distractors, and it must tolerate variability in the appearance of the target. The present experiment examined how the tolerance of the search template is affected by the search task. Two groups of 18 observers trained on the same set of stimuli blocked either by target image (block-by-image group) or by target category (block-by-category group). One or two days after training, both groups were tested on a related search task. The pattern of test results revealed that the two groups of observers had developed different search templates, and that the templates of the block-by-category observers better captured the general characteristics of the category. These results demonstrate that observers match their search templates to the demands of the search task. 相似文献
108.
Choosing and using geospatial displays: effects of design on performance and metacognition 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Interactive display systems give users flexibility to tailor their visual displays to different tasks and situations. However, in order for such flexibility to be beneficial, users need to understand how to tailor displays to different tasks (to possess "metarepresentational competence"). Recent research suggests that people may desire more complex and realistic displays than are most effective (Smallman & St. John, 2005). In Experiment 1, undergraduate students were tested on a comprehension task with geospatial displays (weather maps) that varied in the number of extraneous variables displayed. Their metacognitive judgments about the relative effectiveness of the displays were also solicited. Extraneous variables slowed response time and increased errors, but participants favored complex maps that looked more realistic about one third of the time. In Experiment 2, the eye fixations of undergraduate students were monitored as they performed the comprehension task. Complex maps that looked more realistic led to more eye fixations on both task-relevant and task-irrelevant regions of the displays. Experiment 3 compared performance of experienced meteorologists and undergraduate students on the comprehension and metacognitive tasks. Meteorologists were as likely as undergraduate students to prefer geographically complex (realistic) displays and more likely than undergraduates to opt for displays that added extraneous weather variables. However, meteorologists were also slower and less accurate with complex than with simple displays. This work highlights the importance of empirically testing principles of visual display design and suggests some limits to metarepresentational competence. 相似文献
109.
Garland AF Haine-Schlagel R Accurso EC Baker-Ericzén MJ Brookman-Frazee L 《Psychological services》2012,9(1):74-88
Sustained treatment attendance has been reported to be poor in publicly funded community-based clinic settings serving children and families. Several child and family characteristics have been shown to predict attendance in community-based care, but virtually no research has been conducted to examine how experiences in care, including psychotherapists' within-session practices, influence client attendance. The goal of this exploratory study was to examine how observed practice within sessions, in particular the extent to which therapists delivered elements consistent with evidence-based practices, impacts total number of sessions attended, while accounting for an array of other potential predictors. Participants include 181 children ages 4-13 and their parents entering a new episode of care for disruptive behavior problems in publicly funded clinics. Data sources include administrative billing records on treatment attendance; coded videotaped treatment sessions; and self-reports from children, parents, and therapists. Results indicate that parent education, service funding source, parent alliance with therapist, and therapist experience predicted number of sessions attended; intensity of evidence-based treatment techniques delivered to children was marginally associated with attendance (p = .059). Implications for improving engagement in community-based care are discussed. 相似文献
110.
Aimee Harris T. Mary Foster Joshua Levine William Temple 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2012,98(3):295-309
Domestic hens responded under multiple fixed‐ratio fixed‐ratio schedules with equal fixed ratios. One component provided immediate reinforcement and the other provided reinforcement after a delay, signaled by the offset of the key light. The components were presented quasirandomly so that all four possible transitions occurred in each session. The delay was varied over 0, 4, 8, 16, and 32 s with fixed‐ratio 5 schedules, and over 0, 8 and 32 s with fixed‐ratio 1, 15 and 40 schedules. Main effects of fixed‐ratio value and delay duration were detected on between‐ratio pauses. Pauses were longer when the multiple‐schedule stimulus correlated with a delayed‐reinforcer component was presented, with the longest pauses occurring at the transition from a component with an immediate reinforcer to one with a delayed reinforcer. Pause durations were shortest during immediate components. Overall, both the presence or absence of a delay in the upcoming component, and the presence or absence of a delay in the preceding component affected pause length, but the upcoming delay had the larger effect. Thus changes in delay had similar effects to past reports of the effects of changes in response force, response requirement, and reinforcer magnitude in multiple fixed‐ratio fixed‐ratio schedules. 相似文献