首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3196篇
  免费   105篇
  国内免费   3篇
  2021年   22篇
  2020年   41篇
  2019年   61篇
  2018年   91篇
  2017年   78篇
  2016年   85篇
  2015年   57篇
  2014年   66篇
  2013年   397篇
  2012年   112篇
  2011年   120篇
  2010年   87篇
  2009年   73篇
  2008年   107篇
  2007年   110篇
  2006年   103篇
  2005年   103篇
  2004年   114篇
  2003年   98篇
  2002年   103篇
  2001年   51篇
  2000年   46篇
  1999年   57篇
  1998年   61篇
  1997年   34篇
  1996年   63篇
  1995年   55篇
  1994年   56篇
  1993年   51篇
  1992年   55篇
  1991年   36篇
  1990年   42篇
  1989年   41篇
  1988年   34篇
  1987年   44篇
  1986年   36篇
  1985年   52篇
  1984年   34篇
  1983年   47篇
  1982年   40篇
  1981年   34篇
  1980年   35篇
  1979年   22篇
  1978年   26篇
  1977年   25篇
  1976年   20篇
  1975年   17篇
  1974年   28篇
  1973年   19篇
  1971年   14篇
排序方式: 共有3304条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Book reviews     
Kite  Mary  Hall  Lynda  Chrisler  Joan C. 《Sex roles》1994,31(11-12):771-778
  相似文献   
992.
993.
994.
995.
In studies of verbal short-term memory it has been shown that the length of words to be remembered affects the size of memory span. This word-length effect is attributed to relationships between the rate of rehearsal of verbal material and the time it takes to speak the words being rehearsed. For spatial memory span there may also be an internal rehearsal system linked to overt responding, and if there is a strong analogy to be drawn between the verbal and spatial domains then movement time between spatial targets should predict the number of spatial locations that can be recalled. In the experiments reported here the time taken to move between spatial targets is varied by altering the size of targets and the distance between them. No difference between span performance on a nine-block spatial span task were found, either on immediate recall or on recall after an interval. When recall is of items from an array of 27, grouped in nine sets of three, with only one location in any set being presented on any trial, there is an effect of display size. This effect is consonant with the argument that movement time is related to spatial rehearsal, but other explanations are also possible. However, if recall in this task is scored over the nine sets rather than over the 27 items, then there is no difference between the displays. The results indicate that performance on the normal nine-block spatial-span task cannot be predicted by movement time.  相似文献   
996.
We hypothesize that effective collaboration is facilitated when individuals and environmental components form a synergy where they work together and regulate one another to produce stable patterns of behavior, or regularity, as well as adaptively reorganize to form new behaviors, or irregularity. We tested this hypothesis in a study with 32 triads who collaboratively solved a challenging visual computer programming task for 20 min following an introductory warm‐up phase. Multidimensional recurrence quantification analysis was used to examine fine‐grained (i.e., every 10 s) collective patterns of regularity across team members' speech rate, body movement, and team interaction with the shared user interface. We found that teams exhibited significant patterns of regularity as compared to shuffled baselines, but there were no systematic trends in regularity across time. We also found that periods of regularity were associated with a reduction in overall behavior. Notably, the production of irregular behavior predicted expert‐coded metrics of collaborative activity, such as teams' ability to construct shared knowledge and effectively negotiate and coordinate execution of solutions, net of overall behavioral production and behavioral self‐similarity. Our findings support the theory that groups can interact to form interpersonal synergies and indicate that information about system‐level dynamics is a viable way to understand and predict effective collaborative processes.  相似文献   
997.
998.
999.
Mary Jane Haemig 《Dialog》2014,53(4):312-318
Much of theological education and church life has been based on the assumption that people will come to church. Instead, the church needs to look at how it can go out to those who do not come. Careful consideration needs to be given to the traits of leaders needed for this task and the ethos of the education best suited to helping them move out. The past provides some models that may be useful for Lutherans today.  相似文献   
1000.
In the initial interviews of family therapy sessions, the therapist faces the challenge of obtaining and organizing the information that is most relevant toward understanding the essential concerns that families and couples bring to therapy. This article describes the process of clinical interviewing and case conceptualization used in training family therapists at the Ackerman Institute for the Family. This approach helps the therapist bring forward, and organize, specific information into relational hypotheses, or systemic‐relational conceptualizations, that allow both family members and the therapist to understand presenting problems within their relational contexts. While always provisional, relational hypotheses help anchor the therapist in a systemic‐relational frame and provide a conceptual through‐line to guide the ongoing work of the therapy. The process of interviewing and the construction of clear and complex conceptualizations of presenting problems are illustrated through case examples.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号