首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   568篇
  免费   19篇
  587篇
  2023年   7篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   10篇
  2013年   79篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   16篇
  2010年   14篇
  2009年   14篇
  2008年   14篇
  2007年   15篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   19篇
  2003年   19篇
  2002年   19篇
  2001年   6篇
  2000年   11篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   9篇
  1995年   6篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   10篇
  1988年   6篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   6篇
  1983年   11篇
  1982年   4篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   11篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   10篇
  1976年   9篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   11篇
  1973年   6篇
  1971年   4篇
  1970年   4篇
  1968年   6篇
  1967年   11篇
  1963年   5篇
排序方式: 共有587条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
471.
Indirect memory associations for substance use predict both the concurrent and prospective levels of substance use. These methods assess spontaneous, possibly implicit, and easily accessible associations that predict substance use over direct (explicit) methods of assessment (e.g., outcome expectancies). The present study tested and expanded the application of a coding method for alcohol and marijuana associations on the basis of self-coding of indirect responses (Frigon & Krank, 2009). College students generated free associates to (1) ambiguous words (e.g., draft or weed), (2) situations (e.g., at a party, hanging out with friends), and (3) emotions (having fun, feeling dreamy). Later, participants were shown their responses and were asked to code their responses according to both nonrisk and risk activities, such as alcohol and marijuana use. Self-coded scores were higher than researcher-coded scores, captured the same variance, and improved the prediction of substance use. Self-coding of indirect memory associations provides accurate and efficient prediction of the level of alcohol and marijuana. Self-coding is efficient and may be useful for reducing ambiguities in coding of many different kinds of open-ended responses.  相似文献   
472.
Top‐ranked items were identified during 30‐min free‐operant preference assessments for 9 individuals. Data from each session were analyzed to identify the item (a) that was engaged with first in each session and (b) to which the most responding was allocated after 5 min, 10 min, 15 min, 20 min, and 25 min had elapsed in each session. The results indicated that the first‐engaged item and the 5‐min high‐allocation item predicted the top‐ranked item in 55% and 62% of the sessions, respectively. The results also showed that engagement with the top‐ranked item from the first session decreased across subsequent sessions for 6 of the 9 participants. The implications of the results for brief versus extended stimulus preference assessments are discussed.  相似文献   
473.
474.
475.
476.
477.
478.
479.
A number of models for the analysis of moment structures, such as LISREL, have recently been shown to be capable of being given a particularly simple and economical representation, in terms of the Reticular Action Model (RAM). In contrast to previous treatments, a formal algebraic treatment is provided which shows that RAM directly incorporates many common structural models, including models describing the structure of means. It is also shown here that RAM treats coefficient matrices with patterned inverses simply and generally.  相似文献   
480.
This study examined how tone of voice related to directory assistance operators' s]peed at handling customer calls. Brief (5-s) audio exerpts of telephone operator conversation were presented to judges who rated these on 5 personality dimensions and 5 vocal qualities. When these measures were correlated with a key measure of job performance, namely call duration, results showed that operators who were faster at handling calls were rated as significantly more enthusiastic, sympathetic, confident, professional, and friendly (average r= -.56). The voices of faster operators were also rated as significantly more changing (vs. monotonous) and as containing clearer (vs. slurred) pronunciation. Results are discussed in terms of the role that tone of voice plays in telephone operator-customer interaction, and telephone interactions more generally.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号