全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4895篇 |
免费 | 85篇 |
专业分类
4980篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 50篇 |
2020年 | 74篇 |
2019年 | 101篇 |
2018年 | 129篇 |
2017年 | 146篇 |
2016年 | 139篇 |
2015年 | 108篇 |
2014年 | 144篇 |
2013年 | 582篇 |
2012年 | 241篇 |
2011年 | 238篇 |
2010年 | 129篇 |
2009年 | 141篇 |
2008年 | 173篇 |
2007年 | 199篇 |
2006年 | 176篇 |
2005年 | 137篇 |
2004年 | 158篇 |
2003年 | 139篇 |
2002年 | 114篇 |
2001年 | 70篇 |
2000年 | 78篇 |
1999年 | 84篇 |
1998年 | 60篇 |
1997年 | 53篇 |
1996年 | 42篇 |
1995年 | 46篇 |
1994年 | 42篇 |
1993年 | 47篇 |
1992年 | 43篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 46篇 |
1988年 | 47篇 |
1987年 | 40篇 |
1986年 | 51篇 |
1985年 | 55篇 |
1984年 | 54篇 |
1983年 | 46篇 |
1982年 | 42篇 |
1981年 | 50篇 |
1980年 | 43篇 |
1979年 | 43篇 |
1978年 | 47篇 |
1977年 | 28篇 |
1976年 | 40篇 |
1975年 | 50篇 |
1974年 | 44篇 |
1973年 | 33篇 |
1972年 | 30篇 |
排序方式: 共有4980条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
The mediaeval logic of Aristotelian privation, represented by Ockham's expositionof All A is non-P as All S is of a type T that is naturally P and no S is P, iscritically evaluated as an account of privative negation. It is argued that there aretwo senses of privative negation: (1) an intensifier (as in subhuman), the dualof Neoplatonic hypernegation (superhuman), which is studied in linguistics asan operator on scalar adjectives, and (2) a (often lexicalized) Boolean complementrelative to the extension of a privative negation in sense (1) (e.g., Brute). Thissecond sense, which is the privative negation discussed in modern linguistics, isshown to be Aristotle's. It is argued that Ockham's exposition fails to capture muchof the logic of Aristotelian privation due to limitations in the expressive power of thesyllogistic. 相似文献
232.
Weak coherence,no theory of mind,or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Deficits in pragmatic language ability are common to a number of clinical populations, for example, right-hemisphere damage (RHD), Autism and traumatic brain injury (TBI). In these individuals the basic structural components of language may be intact, but the ability to use language to engage socially is impaired. Despite the nature of these difficulties being well documented, exactly what causes these difficulties is less clear. Furthermore, the current status of causal explanations for pragmatic difficulties across these populations is divergent and sometimes contradictory. This paper explores the empirical validity of three theories that attempt to explain pragmatic language impairment. It is recommended that a new, more convergent approach to investigating the causes of pragmatic language disability be adopted. 相似文献
233.
It is well established that motion aftereffects (MAEs) can show interocular transfer (IOT); that is, motion adaptation in one eye can give a MAE in the other eye. Different quantification methods and different test stimuli have been shown to give different IOT magnitudes, varying from no to almost full IOT. In this study, we examine to what extent IOT of the dynamic MAE (dMAE), that is the MAE seen with a dynamic noise test pattern, varies with velocity of the adaptation stimulus. We measured strength of dMAE by a nulling method. The aftereffect induced by adaptation to a moving random-pixel array was compensated (nulled), during a brief dynamic test period, by the same kind of motion stimulus of variable luminance signal-to-noise ratio (LSNR). The LSNR nulling value was determined in a Quest-staircase procedure. We found that velocity has a strong effect on the magnitude of IOT for the dMAE. For increasing speeds from 1.5 deg s(-1) to 24 deg s(-1) average IOT values increased about linearly from 18% to 63% or from 32% to 83%, depending on IOT definition. The finding that dMAEs transfer to an increasing extent as speed increases, suggests that binocular cells play a more dominant role at higher speeds. 相似文献
234.
Staci Martin Pamela L. Wolters Andrea Baldwin Marie Claire Roderick Mary Anne Toledo-Tamula Andrea Gillespie Brigitte Widemann 《Journal of genetic counseling》2014,23(5):796-804
Youth with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) have multiple, complex symptoms associated with physical, social-emotional, and cognitive difficulties. In addition, caring for a child with NF1 can be extremely challenging for parents. Since research with other chronic illness populations suggests that social support, including internet support groups (ISGs), can be beneficial, this survey study aimed to determine the attitudes and preferences of adolescents and young adults with NF1 and parents of a child with NF1 regarding ISGs. Thirty patients and 30 caregivers completed a 24-item survey about ISGs. Many patients and parents are not aware of any ISGs for NF1, but are interested in using one in the future for a variety of reasons, including to get answers to their questions about NF1, to find out about research studies, and to discuss problems and concerns about NF1. Specific concerns of interest include physical, social-emotional, and cognitive aspects of NF1. ISGs have potential as a social support intervention within the NF1 community. ISGs for the NF1 population should include patients with NF1 (or parents of children with NF1) as well as a health professional, and both chat rooms and discussion boards likely would be well-received. 相似文献
235.
236.
Andrea L. Nelson Valerie Vorstenbosch Martin M. Antony 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2014,36(1):105-114
To date, research on storm phobia has been limited by a lack of validated self-report measures for evaluating the severity of anxiety, phobic avoidance, and distress associated with storms and severe weather. The current research presents the development and validation of the Storm Fear Questionnaire (SFQ), a 15-item self-report questionnaire that assesses the cognitive, affective, and behavioral aspects thought to be associated with Storm Phobia in adults. Three studies were conducted to assess 1) the factor structure, internal consistency, and convergent and discriminant validity of the SFQ, 2) the test-retest reliability of the SFQ, 3) the extent that scores on the SFQ were associated with subjective anxiety ratings during a Behavioral Approach Test (BAT) involving exposure to a virtual thunderstorm, and the extent to which SFQ scores distinguished community participants with versus without a fear of storms. Exploratory factor analyses supported a one-factor model with good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α?=?.95), good convergent and discriminant validity with self-report measures of anxiety, worry, depression, and other specific phobias, and acceptable test-retest reliability. Moreover, there was a significant positive association between SFQ scores and anxiety ratings following the BAT involving exposure to a virtual thunderstorm and participants with a high fear of storms reported significantly higher SFQ scores than those with a low fear of storms. In sum, the SFQ has good psychometric properties and appears to be a valuable tool for assessing the severity of fear associated with storms and severe weather. Research evaluating the diagnostic and clinical utility of the SFQ is still needed. 相似文献
237.
238.
A consequential ideology in Western society is the uncontested belief that a committed relationship is the most important adult relationship and that almost all people want to marry or seriously couple (DePaulo & Morris, 2005). In the present article, we investigated the extent to which the system justification motive may contribute to the adoption of this ideology. In Studies 1 and 2, we examined whether a heightened motive to maintain the status quo would increase defense of committed relationship values. In Study 3, we examined the reverse association, that is, whether a threat to committed relationship ideology would also affect sociopolitical system endorsement. As past research has found that the justification of political systems depends upon how much these systems are perceived as controlling, in Study 4 we tested whether the defense of the system of committed relationships would also increase when framed as controlling. Results from Studies 1-4 were consistent with our hypotheses, but only for men. In Study 5, using cross-cultural data, we sought to replicate these findings correlationally and probe for a cause of the gender effect. Results from more than 33,000 respondents indicated a relationship (for men) between defense of the sociopolitical system and defense of marriage in countries where the traditional advantages of men over women were most threatened. In Studies 6 and 7, we investigated when this gender difference disappears. Results revealed that when we measured (Study 6) or manipulated (Study 7) personal relationship identity rather than relationship ideology, effects also emerge for women. 相似文献
239.
Social animals have much to gain from observing and responding appropriately to the actions of their conspecific group members.
This can in turn lead to the learning of novel behavior patterns (social learning) or to foraging, ranging, or social behavioral
choices copied from fellow group members, which do not necessarily result in long-term learning, but at the time represent
adaptive responses to environmental cues (public information use). In the current study, we developed a novel system for the
study of public information use under fully automated conditions. We modified a classic single-subject laboratory paradigm—matching-to-sample
(MTS)—and examined chimpanzees’ ability to interpret and utilize cues provided by the behavior of a conspecific to solve the
task. In Experiment 1, two subjects took turns on an identity MTS task, with one subject (the model) performing the first
half of the trial and the other subject (the observer) completing the trial using the model’s actions as discriminative cues.
Both subjects performed above chance from the first session onwards. In Experiment 2, the subjects were tested on a symbolic
version of the same MTS task, with one subject showing spontaneous transfer. Our study establishes a novel method for examining
public information use within a highly controlled and automated setting. 相似文献
240.
Martin Kusch 《Erkenntnis》2011,75(3):483-494
This paper tries to motivate three desiderata for historical epistemologies: (a) that they should be reflective about the
pedigree of their conceptual apparatus; (b) that they must face up to the potentially relativistic consequences of their historicism;
and (c) that they must not forget the hard-won lessons of microhistory (i.e. historical events must be explained causally;
historical events must not be artificially divided into internal/intellectual and external/social “factors” or “levels”; and
constructed series of homogenous events must not be treated as quasi-organisms). Ian Hacking’s work on styles of reasoning
and Lorraine Daston’s and Peter Galison’s investigation into epistemic virtues are used to identify the costs of neglecting
these desiderata. 相似文献