首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1377篇
  免费   67篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   22篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   30篇
  2020年   42篇
  2019年   36篇
  2018年   63篇
  2017年   58篇
  2016年   61篇
  2015年   25篇
  2014年   41篇
  2013年   154篇
  2012年   66篇
  2011年   62篇
  2010年   39篇
  2009年   46篇
  2008年   48篇
  2007年   55篇
  2006年   44篇
  2005年   33篇
  2004年   33篇
  2003年   17篇
  2002年   37篇
  2001年   24篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   16篇
  1998年   10篇
  1997年   12篇
  1996年   11篇
  1993年   8篇
  1992年   17篇
  1991年   23篇
  1990年   10篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   13篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   17篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   11篇
  1982年   8篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   12篇
  1978年   10篇
  1977年   9篇
  1975年   14篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   13篇
  1972年   8篇
  1971年   8篇
  1966年   15篇
排序方式: 共有1445条查询结果,搜索用时 32 毫秒
941.
Given the growing interest in the study of subjective well-being as a measure of social progress, instruments that produce valid and reliable scores and that can be used within and across countries are needed. The aim of the present study was to analyze the measurement equivalence of the Day Reconstruction Method in its brief version, using nationally representative samples from Finland, Poland, and Spain obtained within the COURAGE in Europe project. The goodness-of-fit of a two-correlated-factors model and the reliability of the scores obtained were assessed. Cross-country invariance was tested employing a multiple group confirmatory factor analysis, through sequential constraint imposition. In each country, measurement invariance was tested across time frames (morning, afternoon and evening) and days of the week (weekday and weekend). The results found support for the hypothesis of a two-correlated-factors (positive and negative affect) structure; the reliability of the positive, the negative and the net affect scores showed appropriate values. A high equivalence across the three national samples was found: all items except one showed strong measurement invariance indicating that respondents from Finland, Poland, and Spain attribute the same meaning to the latent construct under study, and the levels of the underlying items are equal in all three countries. Similar results were found for the measurement equivalence across time frames and days of the week. Our findings support the assumption of comparability across the different samples considered; in general, higher positive affect and lower negative affect were found in Finland, in the evening and at the weekend.  相似文献   
942.
Although women today excel in many areas of society, they are often underrepresented in the traditionally male‐dominated fields of Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math (STEM). The present research examined whether traditional romantic partner preferences—specifically, a desire to date partners who are smarter than oneself—affects women's tendency to minimize their intelligence in STEM fields when pursuing romantic goals. Women (but not men) who preferred smarter romantic partners showed worse math performance (Studies 1–2), less identification with math (Study 2), and less interest in STEM careers (Study 3) when the goal to be romantically desirable was activated. A meta‐analysis across studies supported results. This research thus demonstrates that partner preferences influence women's STEM outcomes in response to romantic goal pursuit.  相似文献   
943.
944.
945.
946.
The effect of a 6‐week loving‐kindness meditation (LKM) on the multidimensional empathy of 103 master's‐level counseling students was evaluated, in addition to the correlation between reported levels of time spent meditating and empathy. Statistical analyses indicated that participants who received the LKM intervention experienced gains in dimensions of empathy. A significant relationship between quantity of meditation and perspective taking was noted. Implications and suggestions for future research are explored.  相似文献   
947.
948.
Tasia Young  Mary B. Harris 《Sex roles》1996,35(5-6):363-375
In order to understand how gender roles influence the selection of people who are admired, we analyzed the results of the Most Admired Men and Women polls conducted annually by Gallup and Good Housekeeping. Most Admired Men were more likely than women to have achieved their fame without sponsorship of a well-known family or spouse. However, the proportion of Autonomous women was higher in the 1980s and 1990s than previously. Men were more likely to be admired for their service in the military, politics, religion, and economics; women were more likely to be admired because they belonged to a royal or political family, or because of their work as activists and reformers or in the area of entertainment and culture. A small percentage of both men and women had been the victim of personal tragedy. The great majority of the admired individuals were White Americans. No Hispanics or Native Americans made the lists, and all but one of the Asians were women. In short, both gender and ethnic stereotypes seem to have influenced the choice of Most Admired Men and Women.  相似文献   
949.
In the liteature, the clinical management of adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder has focused almost exclusively on stimulant medication. Yet psychological therapy may be a useful adjunct to stimulant medication. Pharmacotherapy is generally reported to be efficacious and, as such, treated individuals are likely to be more receptive to psychological intervention. The aim of this paper is to consider how an individual may best be supported by psychological therapy. It is recommended that a structured pragmatic, psychoeducative approach is the most appropriate. The development of specific self-management skills is discussed within a cognitive behavioural framework. Family, marital and group therapies are also discussed.  相似文献   
950.
Skin Conductance Responses (SCRs) to familiar and unfamiliar names and faces were recorded from independent groups of subjects, using two different presentation designs: the first employing fewer familiar than unfamiliar items (Tranel, Fowles, and Damasio, 1985) and the second employing equal numbers of familiar and unfamiliar items. In both designs, familiar faces were responded to significantly more strongly than unfamiliar faces, whereas for names there was no difference in responses to familiar and unfamiliar stimuli. Faces produced significantly larger overall SCRs than names in the unequal familiar/unfamiliar ratio design, but this effect was not observed in the equal ratio design. The results are discussed with particular reference to those previously published by Tranel et al. (1985) and in relation to work both on covert recognition in prosopagnosia and on one of the delusional misidentification syndromes known as the Capgras delusion. This study was carried out with generous support from the EJLB Foundation (Canada) and the Wellcome Trust.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号