首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   337篇
  免费   3篇
  2021年   3篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   8篇
  2017年   6篇
  2016年   6篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   2篇
  2013年   19篇
  2012年   12篇
  2011年   10篇
  2010年   6篇
  2009年   8篇
  2008年   8篇
  2007年   17篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   10篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   8篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   8篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   4篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   4篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   5篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   5篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   4篇
  1971年   3篇
  1969年   2篇
排序方式: 共有340条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
Working memory resources are needed for processing and maintenance of information during cognitive tasks. Many models have been developed to capture the effects of limited working memory resources on performance. However, most of these models do not account for the finding that different individuals show different sensitivities to working memory demands, and none of the models predicts individual subjects' patterns of performance. We propose a computational model that accounts for differences in working memory capacity in terms of a quantity called source activation , which is used to maintain goal-relevant information in an available state. We apply this model to capture the working memory effects of individual subjects at a fine level of detail across two experiments. This, we argue, strengthens the interpretation of source activation as working memory capacity.  相似文献   
62.
63.
Multivariate correlational methods such as P-technique factor analysis provide a way to structure intraindividual change by identifying dimensions of covariation in intensive longitudinal measurements. For example, for 20 years or more researchers have acknowledged that some major personality constructs such as anxiety exhibit both trait (relatively stable, interindividual) and state (intraindividual) variability. However, the consideration that state variability might be quite general and can “intrude” in the measurement of putatively more stable attributes has not generally entered into construct validation work and other manifestations of theoretical development. Locus of control, as with many personality constructs, is considered to be a dimension of relatively stable interindividual differences. Levenson's three-dimensional locus of control scale was adapted for use in a measurement-intensive P-technique study involving 10 participants (5 mothers and fathers who were expecting their first child) to examine the extent to which responses manifested systematic liability. The data indicated that perceived locus of control exhibits coherent, day-to-day variability; especially the dimensions identified as internality and powerful others. Such state variation manifestations are discussed in relation to their important implications for measurement, long-term prediction, and theorizing about the nature of personality.  相似文献   
64.
65.
66.
From a study of methods of test construction – criterion-keying, item analytic, factor analytic and using item characteristic curves – it is shown that, in the sphere of personality tests, only factor analytic methods can ensure univariate measures. A further scrutiny of factor analytic methodology reveals various technical defects which have created a chaos of different findings and factors in personality inventories. It is finally demonstrated that rotation to simple structure yields reliable and replicable factors and that when this is done, four or five factors account for most of the variance in personality inventories. It is concluded that in occupational testing only tests measuring these factors, or primary factors clearly related to them, should be used.  相似文献   
67.
68.
69.
Psychiatric diagnoses and objective parent checklists are alternative ways to describe child adjustment problems. There has recently been interest in evaluating the degree of agreement or convergence between these sources of information. This paper addresses three issues neglected by researchers in this area. The appropriateness of the use of indices of sensitivity and specificity to describe the correspondence of diagnoses and checklist scores is questioned. Implications of failure to consider the reliability of diagnoses in interpreting diagnosis-checklist agreement are discussed. Also, possible parameters of diagnosis-checklist agreement that should be identified by researchers are reviewed. Suggestions for improving research in this area are offered.  相似文献   
70.
Foveal and peripheral target detection were compared in young adults (M age = 22 years) and older adults (M age = 66 years) who were optically corrected for the viewing distance. In a two-alternative, forced-choice task, target letters were presented at 0 degree to 10.5 degrees from fixation. Targets were presented alone, flanked on each side by one noise element (i.e., nontarget letter), or embedded in a horizontal row of 19 noise elements. An Age X Noise Level X Location interaction was obtained, wherein age differences were largest for peripheral targets presented in noise. Slope analyses of latency data showed that the performance of young adults in the high-noise condition was most similar to that of older adults in the low-noise condition. At the functional level, results indicated that aging is associated with a restricted useful field of view. In addition, the data suggest that age differences in search can be described by a model in which older adults take smaller perceptual samples from the visual scene and scan these samples more slowly than do the young adults.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号