全文获取类型
收费全文 | 153篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
157篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 5篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 5篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 8篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有157条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Personality as a moderator in the relations between perceptions of organizational justice and sickness absence 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This longitudinal study examined whether hostility and neuroticism moderated the effect of organizational justice perceptions on short-term sickness absence among 506 male and 3570 female hospital employees. Organizational justice perceptions were assessed using a questionnaire on procedural and relational justice. Self-certified sickness absence records at baseline and during the two-year follow-up were derived from employers’ registers. Hierarchical moderated Poisson regression models suggest that low procedural justice perceptions related to increased sickness absence more in hostile men than in other male employees. Low relational justice perceptions were a greater risk for sickness absence for male employees with higher neuroticism than for their colleagues with lower neuroticism. 相似文献
62.
Instead of assessing activation in distinct brain regions, approaches to investigating the networks underlying distinct brain functions have come into the focus of neuroscience research. Here, we provide a completely data-driven framework for assessing functional and causal connectivity in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data, employing Granger's causality. We investigate the networks underlying story processing in 17 healthy children (8f, 9m, 10.4+/-2.8 years, 6.5-15.4 years). Extensive functional connectivity exists between brain regions, including some not detected in standard random effects analyses. Causal connectivity analyses demonstrate a clear dominance of left-sided language regions for both forward and backward interactions with other network nodes. We believe our approach to be useful in helping to assess language networks in the normal or pathological setting; it may also aid in providing better starting estimates for the more hypothesis-driven approaches like structural equation or dynamic causal modeling. 相似文献
63.
Geraerts E Arnold MM Lindsay DS Merckelbach H Jelicic M Hauer B 《Psychological science》2006,17(11):1002-1008
Case studies of individuals reporting recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse suggest that some overestimate their prior forgetting of the abuse. People reporting recovered or continuous memories of childhood sexual abuse and control subjects reporting no history of abuse participated in two experiments examining this "forgot it all along" phenomenon. Participants in Experiment 1 were more likely to forget that they had previously recalled a studied item if they were cued to think of it differently on two recall tests than if they were cued to think of it in the same way on the two tests. This effect was stronger for recovered-memory participants than for continuous-memory and control participants. In Experiment 2, participants recalled autobiographical events three times over a period of 4 months. Much as in Experiment 1, they underestimated prior remembering when the events had been recalled in a different emotional frame (positive vs. negative) on the previous occasion. This underestimation was more pronounced for recovered-memory participants than for continuous-memory and control participants. 相似文献
64.
New insights in trauma and memory: a special issue of memory 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
65.
Kurt Marko 《Studies in East European Thought》1982,24(1):43-44
Ohne Zusammenfassung 相似文献
66.
Jessie Bullens Marko Nardini Christian F. Doeller Oliver Braddick Albert Postma Neil Burgess 《Developmental science》2010,13(1):170-180
It has been suggested that learning an object's location relative to (1) intramaze landmarks and (2) local boundaries is supported by parallel striatal and hippocampal systems, both of which rely upon input from a third system for orientation. However, little is known about the developmental trajectories of these systems' contributions to spatial learning. The present study tested 5- and 7-year-old children and adults on a water maze-like task in which all three types of cue were available. Participants had to remember the location of an object hidden in a circular bounded environment containing a moveable intramaze landmark and surrounded by distal cues. Children performed less accurately than adults, and showed a different pattern of error. While adults relied most on the stable cue provided by the boundary, children relied on both landmark and boundary cues similarly, suggesting a developmental increase in the weighting given to boundary cues. Further, adults were most accurate in coding angular information (dependent on distal cues), whereas children were most accurate in coding distance, suggesting a developing ability to use distal cues to orient. These results indicate that children as young as 5 years use boundary, intramaze landmark, and distal visual cues in parallel, but that the basic accuracy and relative weighting of these cues changes during subsequent development. 相似文献
67.
Laura Pulkki-R?back Marko Elovainio Mika Kivim?ki Olli T Raitakari Liisa Keltikangas-J?rvinen 《Health psychology》2005,24(3):307-315
This study examined associations of temperament at ages 6 to 12 with body-mass index (BMI) and waist circumference (WC) at ages 24 to 30 years. The participants were 619 men and women derived from the population-based Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Temperament was operationalized as (negative) emotionality, sociability, and activity. High emotionality predicted increased BMI, independently of WC, and independently of childhood and adulthood risk factors for adult obesity. None of the temperament dimensions had any associations with WC after controlling for BMI. The findings suggest that temperamental difficulty in childhood may be a useful risk indicator for general body mass in adulthood, and the mechanisms relating temperament with body mass should be further explored. 相似文献
68.
Fischer A Radulovic M Schrick C Sananbenesi F Godovac-Zimmermann J Radulovic J 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2007,87(1):149-158
Fear memories elicit multiple behavioral responses, encompassing avoidance, or behavioral inhibition in response to threatening contexts. Context-specific freezing, reflecting fear-induced behavioral inhibition, has been proposed as one of the main risks factors for the development of anxiety disorders. We attempted to define the key hippocampal mediators of extinction in a mouse model of context-dependent freezing. Nine-week-old male C57BL/6J mice were trained and tested for contextual fear conditioning and extinction. Freezing behavior scored by unbiased sampling, was used as an index of fear. Proteomic, immunoblot, and immunohistochemical approaches were employed to identify, verify, and analyze the alterations of the hippocampal extracellular signal-regulated kinases 1 and 2 (Erk-1/2). Targeted pharmacological inhibition of the Erk-1/2 activating kinase, the mitogen activated and extracellular signal-regulated kinase (Mek), served to establish the role of Mek/Erk signaling in extinction. When compared to acquisition, extinction of contextual freezing triggered a rapid activation of Erk-1/2 showing a distinctive time-course, nuclear localization, and subcellular isoform distribution. These differences suggested that the upstream regulation and downstream effects of this pathway might be specific for each process. Dorsohippocampal injections of the Mek inhibitors U0126 (0.5 microg/site) and PD98059 (1.5 microg/site) immediately after the nonreinforced trials prevented Erk-1/2 activation and significantly impaired extinction. This effect was dissociable from potential actions on memory retrieval or reconsolidation. On the basis of these findings, we propose that hippocampal Mek/Erk signaling might serve as one of the key mediators of contextual fear extinction. 相似文献
69.
Philosophical Studies - Peter Singer’s defense of the duty to aid the world’s poor by the pond analogy is self-defeating. It cannot be both true that you ought to save the drowning... 相似文献
70.
Authorship in a small medical journal: A study of contributorship statements by corresponding authors 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Marusić M Bozikov J Katavić V Hren D Kljaković-Gaspić M Marusić A 《Science and engineering ethics》2004,10(3):493-502
The authorship criteria of the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) are widely accepted in biomedical
journals, but many studies in large and prestigious journals show that a considerable proportion of authors do not fulfill
these criteria. We investigated authorship contributions in a small medical journal outside the scientific mainstream, to
see if poor adherence to authorship criteria is common in biomedical journals. We analyzed statements on research contribution,
as checked by the corresponding author, for individual authors of 114 research articles, representing 475 authors, submitted
to the Croatian Medical Journal (CMJ) from 1999 to 2000. Only 40% of authors fulfilled the ICMJE authorship criteria. The
authors listed first on the by-line were more likely to fulfill the authorship criteria than all other authors on the by-line.
The percentage of authors fulfilling the ICMJE criteria of authorship decreased with the increase in the number of authors
listed on the by-line. These results indicate that poor adherence to ICMJE authorship criteria is poor across biomedical journals,
regardless of the size of the scientific community. Authorship and contributorship in biomedical journals, as well as editorial
ethical responsibilities towards authorship criteria need critical redefinition and education of both editors and authors. 相似文献