全文获取类型
收费全文 | 9678篇 |
免费 | 135篇 |
专业分类
9813篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 52篇 |
2023年 | 83篇 |
2022年 | 69篇 |
2021年 | 120篇 |
2020年 | 192篇 |
2019年 | 223篇 |
2018年 | 294篇 |
2017年 | 291篇 |
2016年 | 367篇 |
2015年 | 259篇 |
2014年 | 259篇 |
2013年 | 1062篇 |
2012年 | 458篇 |
2011年 | 546篇 |
2010年 | 322篇 |
2009年 | 352篇 |
2008年 | 425篇 |
2007年 | 429篇 |
2006年 | 354篇 |
2005年 | 311篇 |
2004年 | 349篇 |
2003年 | 353篇 |
2002年 | 293篇 |
2001年 | 183篇 |
2000年 | 179篇 |
1999年 | 158篇 |
1998年 | 146篇 |
1997年 | 116篇 |
1996年 | 101篇 |
1995年 | 99篇 |
1994年 | 106篇 |
1993年 | 69篇 |
1992年 | 108篇 |
1991年 | 66篇 |
1990年 | 96篇 |
1989年 | 74篇 |
1988年 | 60篇 |
1987年 | 58篇 |
1986年 | 65篇 |
1985年 | 66篇 |
1984年 | 55篇 |
1983年 | 70篇 |
1982年 | 57篇 |
1981年 | 44篇 |
1980年 | 37篇 |
1979年 | 54篇 |
1978年 | 55篇 |
1977年 | 33篇 |
1976年 | 30篇 |
1972年 | 23篇 |
排序方式: 共有9813条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
241.
242.
243.
244.
ABSTRACTIn this research we investigated whether creativity and career assessment are a contradiction in terms or whether, through creative thinking, their potential complementarity in career guidance and counselling can be realised. The research demonstrates the creative application of the systemic Integrative Structured Interview (ISI) for the Self-Directed Search (SDS) with a South African female mid-life career changer. Results indicated that the ISI positions clients as storytellers who create rich systemic stories about their quantitative SDS scores. The constructs of story telling are evident throughout an interview about quantitative scores; the scores become unifying themes across the stories. In sum, the ISI can assist mid-life career changers to make meaning of their career transition; and by embedding career assessment in a systemic story telling process creativity is evidenced as a multilayered process. 相似文献
245.
This paper reports on a study of the relationship between demographic, personality, and attitudinal variables and impulsive buying (a consumer's tendency to buy spontaneously, unreflectively, and immediately); using secondary analysis of data from common participants in two large national surveys of British adults: one survey contributing data on impulsive buying, demographics, and money attitudes; and the second, a Big Five personality trait measure. In particular, we focus on the attitudes characterized by the extent to which individuals associate money with security, freedom, power, and love. Younger females and those with higher household income were more likely to engage in impulsive buying. Correlational and regression analysis showed that those high on Neuroticism and Extraversion and those low on Conscientiousness were more likely to be impulse buyers. All four money attitudes were related to impulsive buying (Money as Security most strongly). A hierarchical regression indicated that demographic variables accounted for 3 percent, personality a further 9 percent, and money attitudes a further 13 percent of the variance, showing that these three sets of variables accounted for around a quarter of the variance. Implications are considered for educational and therapeutic interventions in reducing maladaptive impulsive buying. 相似文献
246.
247.
248.
Selina N. Emhardt Ellen M. Kok Halszka Jarodzka Saskia Brand-Gruwel Christian Drumm Tamara van Gog 《Cognitive Science》2020,44(9):e12893
Domain experts regularly teach novice students how to perform a task. This often requires them to adjust their behavior to the less knowledgeable audience and, hence, to behave in a more didactic manner. Eye movement modeling examples (EMMEs) are a contemporary educational tool for displaying experts’ (natural or didactic) problem-solving behavior as well as their eye movements to learners. While research on expert-novice communication mainly focused on experts’ changes in explicit, verbal communication behavior, it is as yet unclear whether and how exactly experts adjust their nonverbal behavior. This study first investigated whether and how experts change their eye movements and mouse clicks (that are displayed in EMMEs) when they perform a task naturally versus teach a task didactically. Programming experts and novices initially debugged short computer codes in a natural manner. We first characterized experts’ natural problem-solving behavior by contrasting it with that of novices. Then, we explored the changes in experts’ behavior when being subsequently instructed to model their task solution didactically. Experts became more similar to novices on measures associated with experts’ automatized processes (i.e., shorter fixation durations, fewer transitions between code and output per click on the run button when behaving didactically). This adaptation might make it easier for novices to follow or imitate the expert behavior. In contrast, experts became less similar to novices for measures associated with more strategic behavior (i.e., code reading linearity, clicks on run button) when behaving didactically. 相似文献
249.
Milou van Harsel Vincent Hoogerheide Peter Verkoeijen Tamara van Gog 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(4):793-812
Research suggests some sequences of examples and problems (i.e., EE, EP) are more effective (higher test performance) and efficient (attained with equal/less mental effort) than others (PP, sometimes also PE). Recent findings suggest this is due to motivational variables (i.e., self-efficacy), but did not test this during the training phase. Moreover, prior research used only short task sequences. Therefore, we investigated effects on motivational variables, effectiveness, and efficiency in a short (Experiment 1; four learning tasks; n = 157) and longer task sequence (Experiment 2; eight learning tasks; n = 105). With short sequences, all example conditions were more effective, efficient, and motivating than PP. With longer sequences, all example conditions were more motivating and efficient than PP, but only EE was more effective than PP. Moreover, EE was most efficient during training, regardless of sequence length. These results suggest that example study (only) is more effective, efficient, and more motivating than PP. 相似文献
250.
Nathan Ryan Nina Westera Mark Kebbell Rebecca Milne Mark Harrison 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(3):565-576
Missing body homicide cases have gained public interest globally due to some high-profile cases. In many of these cases, the task of locating the victim's remains relies on the information investigators can gain through the interviewing of willing suspects. To date, investigative interviewing research has largely focused on the retrieval of episodic memory (events) without focusing on spatial memory, a prominent cognitive task required in locating a victim's remains. The current experiment tests the enhanced cognitive interview (ECI) against a free recall strategy in a mock homicide scenario where participants are required to hide and retrieve an object in a natural bushland setting. The results showed that those in the ECI condition produced more coarse- and fine-grained details of landmarks and their actions at and journeying to the deposition site. This demonstrates the value of using the ECI in generating more valuable information to assist in successive search attempts. 相似文献