首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   6227篇
  免费   260篇
  2023年   45篇
  2022年   33篇
  2021年   50篇
  2020年   123篇
  2019年   119篇
  2018年   165篇
  2017年   160篇
  2016年   219篇
  2015年   148篇
  2014年   136篇
  2013年   673篇
  2012年   282篇
  2011年   350篇
  2010年   201篇
  2009年   231篇
  2008年   269篇
  2007年   302篇
  2006年   244篇
  2005年   243篇
  2004年   244篇
  2003年   241篇
  2002年   224篇
  2001年   121篇
  2000年   110篇
  1999年   116篇
  1998年   113篇
  1997年   83篇
  1996年   91篇
  1995年   80篇
  1994年   93篇
  1993年   57篇
  1992年   77篇
  1991年   50篇
  1990年   61篇
  1989年   40篇
  1988年   36篇
  1987年   47篇
  1986年   43篇
  1985年   45篇
  1984年   42篇
  1983年   55篇
  1982年   49篇
  1981年   35篇
  1980年   33篇
  1979年   48篇
  1978年   46篇
  1977年   29篇
  1976年   31篇
  1973年   22篇
  1972年   19篇
排序方式: 共有6487条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
212.
213.
214.
In an experiment to study the effects on attitudes of requiring subjects to use evaluatively biased language, 84 schoolchildren aged 13–14 years completed a questionnaire to measure their attitudes on the issue of adult authority over teenagers, before and after writing an essay on this issue in which they were either required to incorporate words from a list all of which implied a positive evaluation of a pro-authority position or a negative evaluation of an anti-authority position (pro-bias condition), or required to incorporate words where the implied evaluations were reversed (anti-bias condition), or were given no words to incorporate (control condition). Relative to controls, pro-bias subjects showed as a shift towards a more pro position and anti-bias subjects became more anti irrespective of their initial attitudes (pro-bias versus anti-bias comparison, p<.01). However, when tested 6 days later most of this effect had disappeared, particularly in the case of subjects whose initial attitudes were least pro. At this final session, subjects also rated attitude statements on the issue in terms of scales constructed from the pro-bias and anti-bias word lists. In accordance with previous research, the more pro subjects' attitudes, the more they showed greater polarization of judgement on the pro-bias than the anti-bias scales (p<.000l). It is concluded that a person's attitude may be related to the kind of evaluative language he will apply to an issue, and that when a person is induced to use language implying a particular evaluation of an issue, he may change his attitude, at least in the short term, so as to be more congruent with the language he has used.  相似文献   
215.
Self-selected paraprofessional trainees enrolled in a helping skills training program participated in a 6-minute pretraining helping interview from which their frequency of continuing responses was obtained. Continuing responses allow helpees to present their concerns in a nonthreatening, supportive environment and are important in the relationship establishing stage of the helping process. For data analysis purposes only, trainees were divided into three groups, a high, medium, and low group, based on the frequency of continuing responses made. Following training, at posttest, no significant differences were found among the three groups on the amount of continuing responses made during a second identical 6-minute interview. Implications for various selection procedures and training are discussed.  相似文献   
216.
217.
A variety of behavioral procedures have been employed in recent years to modify disruptive classroom behavior. Such methods have been developed with the belief that curtailing disruptive behavior would strengthen positive classroom performance. In this study, two procedures, the good behavior game and the teacher-attention method, were compared to determine short-run effectiveness. Four teachers (two fourth-grade and two fifth-grade) implemented both methods in their classrooms over a five-week period. Presentation of methods was alternated in a counterbalanced design to control for order effects. Each of the four classrooms consisted of 25 students. A time-sampling procedure was used to record the presence or absence of disruptive behavior within 15-second intervals. Disruptive behavior was defined as any talking-out or out-of-seat behavior without permission. The results indicated that both procedures were effective in modifying disruptive classroom behavior, but that the good behavior game reduced disruptive behavior significantly better than the teacher-attention method. In addition, all teachers preferred the game to the teacher-attention procedure. This reaction seemed related to the effort involved in initiating the two activities. The good behavior game required less effort on the teacher's part. However, use of the game alone raises certain ethical considerations. One such issue involves abuse of peer pressure. Also, there is a possibility that negative rules may tend to promote resentment. Positively stated rules would ameliorate that problem. Another relates to the possibility that some teachers might be carried away by the ease of the game's implementation to the extent that behavior control becomes the primary objective in the classroom. As a result, one might consider use of the game to maximize short-term change, but then phase out this procedure in favor of another method (e.g., teacher attention) for long-run effects.  相似文献   
218.
In three studies the formulation was tested that characteristics of the observer have an effect on imitation only in situations which provide little information concerning appropriate or expected behaviors. In Experiment I, female college students of high, medium, and low need for social approval underwent either a high or low arousal manipulation. Subjects observed a videotaped model who engaged in a size judgment task. For half the subjects the model received verbal reward (high information); for the other half the model received no feedback (low information). Subsequently, the model and subject responded alternately in the task during which no feedback was given. Neither need for social approval nor manipulated arousal was found to be systematically related to imitation in either information condition. However, the hypothesized relationships were found when subjects' arousal or anxiety levels were determined by self ratings. These results were replicated in a second, similar experiment. In a third experiment, which involved an improved arousal manipulation, the hypothesized relationships were confirmed.  相似文献   
219.
A device is described which automatically measures barpresses reinforced by short opportunities to attack male mice. Data on barpress rates indicate rapid and reliable acquisition of preferences for reinforcer bars and rapid abolition of this preference when the reinforcement contingency is subsequently eliminated. The data are less reliable for reversal learning.  相似文献   
220.
To offset shortcomings of existing demonstrations of right-ear superiority in the analysis of formant transitions, an experiment was performed on whispered speech. Two aspects of dichotic listening performance were examined in a single-report paradigm: the right-ear advantage (REA) for the perception of the voicing distinction and the feature sharing advantage (FSA) for both voicing and place features. A significant REA was obtained for the voicing distinction cued by first formant transition in the absence of a switch from aperiodic to periodic excitation. This, plus a greater incidence of voiced responses to right-ear stimuli, suggests that a distinction involving transitions can specifically augment the REA. The data also showed better identification of place and of voicing feature values when the competing dichotic speech stimuli shared these respective features (FSA) than when they did not. This FSA was restricted to the feature shared and hence not an effect of response uncertainty. The implications of these results for models of speech processing are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号