全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5615篇 |
免费 | 247篇 |
专业分类
5862篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 28篇 |
2021年 | 47篇 |
2020年 | 112篇 |
2019年 | 110篇 |
2018年 | 151篇 |
2017年 | 146篇 |
2016年 | 202篇 |
2015年 | 140篇 |
2014年 | 122篇 |
2013年 | 610篇 |
2012年 | 258篇 |
2011年 | 320篇 |
2010年 | 188篇 |
2009年 | 220篇 |
2008年 | 240篇 |
2007年 | 286篇 |
2006年 | 223篇 |
2005年 | 217篇 |
2004年 | 229篇 |
2003年 | 230篇 |
2002年 | 202篇 |
2001年 | 110篇 |
2000年 | 99篇 |
1999年 | 103篇 |
1998年 | 105篇 |
1997年 | 79篇 |
1996年 | 79篇 |
1995年 | 76篇 |
1994年 | 86篇 |
1993年 | 56篇 |
1992年 | 72篇 |
1991年 | 42篇 |
1990年 | 51篇 |
1989年 | 32篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 37篇 |
1986年 | 36篇 |
1985年 | 37篇 |
1984年 | 33篇 |
1983年 | 51篇 |
1982年 | 41篇 |
1981年 | 32篇 |
1980年 | 29篇 |
1979年 | 39篇 |
1978年 | 45篇 |
1977年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 28篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有5862条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
44.
The strict competence hypothesis has sparked a small dialogue among several researchers attempting to understand its ramifications for human sentence processing and incremental interpretation in particular. In this paper, we review the dialogue, reconstructing the arguments in an attempt to make them more uniform and crisp, and provide our own analyses of certain of the issues that arise. We argue that strict competence, because it requires a synchronous computation mechanism, may actually lead to more complex, rather than simpler, models of incremental interpretation. Asynchronous computation, which is arguably both psychologically more plausible and conceptually more basic, allows for incremental interpretation to fall out naturally, without additional machinery for interpreting partial constituents. We show that this is true regardless of whether the presumed interpretation mechanism is top-down or bottom-up, contra previous conclusions in the literature, and propose a particular implementation of some of these ideas using a novel representation based on tree-adjoining grammars.The research in this paper was supported in part by grant IRI-9157996 from the National Science Foundation to the first author. The authors would like to thank Fernando Pereira, Edward Stabler, and Mark Steedman for discussions on the topic of this paper and for their comments on previous drafts. 相似文献
45.
46.
A Final Comment on the Case of the Family Environment Scale 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
47.
Thomas H. Ollendick Ross W. Greene Mark D. Weist Donald P. Oswald 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1990,18(6):699-713
The purpose of this study was to examine the predictive validity of a teacher nomination procedure for identifying at- risk children. Two hundred and twenty-five children were nominated by their teachers as well- adjusted (n= 75), socially withdrawn (n=76), or socially aggressive (n=74) during the fourth grade. Five years later, 198 of these children (88%) were located and their adjustment evaluated. Significant differences among the nominated children were found on a variety of measures, including academic grades, sociometric status, and social behavior. In addition, differences in school dropout and delinquent offenses were noted. Results are discussed in terms of the validity of teacher nomination procedures and their utility in identifying at- risk youth. 相似文献
48.
Mark R. Dadds Jeannie K. Sheffield John F. Holbeck 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1990,18(2):121-129
Previous research has tentatively indicated that coercive, ineffective parenting might have a mediating role in the differential impact of marital discord on boys and girls. To further explore this role, we examined the relationship between children's perceptions of marital discord and their evaluations of parental discipline techniques. Ninety-one children aged 8 to 13 years (mean age: 11 years 4 months) were split into high, moderate, and low perceived marital discord on the basis of their responses to the Children's Perceptions Questionnaire (Emery & O'Leary, 1982). The children rated how coercive they believed both mothers and fathers would and should be in three discipline situations. Children with high marital discord indicated that both mothers and fathers would and should use more coercive behavior than did children with low marital discord. The effects of marital discord were stronger for boys than for girls. All children also believed that fathers would and should be more coercive than mothers. Results are discussed in relation to the effects of marital discord on children's social development with regard to sex of the child.Thanks are due to Mike Siegal and Pat Noller for their helpful comments on previous drafts. 相似文献
49.
A 50-state statutory search revealed that variations exist in marital rape exemptions throughout the United States. States handle the question of marital rape in one of three ways: (1) by abolishing the exemption for husbands; (2) by qualifying the exemption; or (3) by allowing the exemption. Although marital rape is a topic that has been largely ignored by social psychology, it is an area ripe for psycho-legal analysis. Possible relationships between social psychology, marital rape exemptions, and privacy are discussed against a background of a psychological jurisprudence. 相似文献
50.
The relational communication of sixteen dropout versus sixteen continuation sessions of solution-focused therapy was studied using two different coding schemes. On the Topic Initiation/Topic Following coding scheme (Tracey, 1986) no differences were found between dropout and continuation sessions, whereas the Family-Relational Communication Control Coding Scheme (Heatherington and Friedlander, 1987) yielded a number of significant findings. Both in 'successful' and in 'unsuccessful' dropout sessions, therapeutic interaction was found to be more conflictive than in continuation cases, with clients showing more domineering behaviour in dropout than in continuation sessions. Markovian and lag sequential analysis are used to clarify some of these findings, and their implications are discussed. 相似文献