首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   13154篇
  免费   568篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   77篇
  2022年   62篇
  2021年   139篇
  2020年   251篇
  2019年   238篇
  2018年   332篇
  2017年   324篇
  2016年   430篇
  2015年   302篇
  2014年   322篇
  2013年   1350篇
  2012年   609篇
  2011年   682篇
  2010年   416篇
  2009年   429篇
  2008年   546篇
  2007年   595篇
  2006年   485篇
  2005年   500篇
  2004年   493篇
  2003年   475篇
  2002年   448篇
  2001年   250篇
  2000年   188篇
  1999年   226篇
  1998年   210篇
  1997年   190篇
  1996年   167篇
  1995年   157篇
  1994年   166篇
  1993年   140篇
  1992年   161篇
  1991年   132篇
  1990年   122篇
  1989年   89篇
  1988年   79篇
  1987年   101篇
  1986年   104篇
  1985年   124篇
  1984年   104篇
  1983年   127篇
  1982年   113篇
  1981年   113篇
  1980年   102篇
  1979年   114篇
  1978年   124篇
  1977年   106篇
  1976年   98篇
  1975年   74篇
  1974年   77篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
13.
14.
This article discusses the meaning of epistemological violence in the empirical social sciences. It is argued that the concept is closer to personal than to structural violence in that it has a subject, an object, and an action, even if the violence is indirect and nonphysical: the subject of violence is the researcher, the object is the Other, and the action is the interpretation of data that is presented as knowledge. Using a hypothetical example, the problem of interpretation in empirical research on the Other is discussed. Epistemological violence refers to the interpretation of social-scientific data on the Other and is produced when empirical data are interpreted as showing the inferiority of or problematizes the Other, even when data allow for equally viable alternative interpretations. Interpretations of inferiority or problematizations are understood as actions that have a negative impact on the Other. Because the interpretations of data emerge from an academic context and thus are presented as knowledge, they are defined as epistemologically violent actions. Problems, consequences, and practices surrounding this concept are discussed.  相似文献   
15.
16.
Drawing on terror management theory (TMT), we discuss the psychological motivations that shape personality at two levels: the characteristically human personality common to us all and the individual differences that distinguish some people from others. TMT posits that the motivation to protect the self against deep-rooted fears about mortality drives people to maintain meaningful, reliable conceptions of reality and positive evaluations of themselves, two broad tendencies that form the foundation of every person’s personality. We review studies showing that mortality reminders increase efforts to bolster cultural sources of meaning and self-esteem in similar ways across individuals and cultures. TMT also posits that individual differences in personality partly reflect the different sources of meaning and self-esteem that people invest in to assuage mortality fears. We review evidence that individual differences predict the degree and direction of people’s defensive responses to mortality reminders. Directions for future research are discussed.  相似文献   
17.
Suicides among older adults in the United States and Canada in the last decade have accounted for an increasing share of all suicides. In both countries the use of firearms among older adults has steadily increased. Despite these trends, little is known about the extent to which crisis centers are prepared to prevent elder suicide. A survey of AAS-listed crisis prevention centers examined the training, knowledge, and current practices relevant to elder suicide among personnel in 321 crisis prevention centers in both countries. Results revealed insufficient training, a lack of familiarity with recent suicide trends, and limited outreach to older adults.  相似文献   
18.
19.
20.
Various lines of research have recently reported links between the ability to recall past and imagine future episodes. Here I report evidence that such links exist in development. Eighty‐two children were asked to report what they did yesterday and what they are going to do tomorrow. Children who could answer one were also more likely to answer the other question. Furthermore, there was an association between the quantity of responses for past and future questions. This correlation continued to be significant even when controlling for children's capacity to answer similar questions that did not have a temporal, episodic component (e.g., ‘tell me all the things that you can think of that are red’). Together, these results are in line with the growing evidence for fundamental links in the neuro‐cognitive resources involved in thinking about past and future events.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号